Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 32
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Brain Struct Funct ; 227(4): 1265-1278, 2022 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35118562

RESUMEN

Most neurons in the primary visual cortex (V1) of mammals show sharp orientation selectivity and band-pass spatial frequency tuning. Here, we examine whether sharpening of the broad tuning that exists subcortically, namely in the retina and the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), underlie the sharper tuning seen for both the above features in tree shrew V1. Since the transition from poor feature selectivity to sharp tuning occurs entirely within V1 in tree shrews, we examined the orientation selectivity and spatial frequency tuning of neurons within individual electrode penetrations. We found that most layer 4 and layer 2/3 neurons in the same cortical column preferred the same stimulus orientation. However, a subset of layer 3c neurons close to the layer 4 border preferred near orthogonal orientations, suggesting that layer 2/3 neurons may inherit the orientation preferences of their layer 4 input neurons and also receive cross-orientation inhibition from layer 3c neurons. We also found that layer 4 neurons showed sharper orientation selectivity at higher spatial frequencies, suggesting that attenuation of low spatial frequency responses by spatially broad inhibition acting on layer 4 inputs to layer 2/3 neurons can enhance both orientation and spatial frequency selectivities. However, in a proportion of layer 2/3 neurons, the sharper tuning of layer 2/3 neurons appeared to arise also or even mainly from inhibition specific to high spatial frequencies acting on the layer 4 inputs to layer 2/3. Overall, our results are consistent with the suggestion that in tree shrews, sharp feature selectivity in layer 2/3 can be established by intracortical mechanisms that sharpen biases observed in layer 4, which are in turn inherited presumably from thalamic afferents.


Asunto(s)
Tupaia , Corteza Visual , Animales , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Corteza Visual Primaria , Tupaiidae , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología
2.
Trends Neurosci ; 45(4): 323-335, 2022 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35190202

RESUMEN

Oscillatory neural activity is believed to have a central role in information processing in the mammalian brain. While early studies often focussed on the function of individual frequency bands, there is emerging appreciation for the role of simultaneous activity in many distinct frequency bands and the interactions between them in high-level cognitive functions. Here, we focus on the role of cross-frequency coupling (CFC) in visual attention. First, we propose a framework that reconciles previous contrasting findings, showing how CFC could have a functional role on both intra- and interareal scales. Second, we outline how CFC between distinct frequency bands could label different submodalities of sensory information. Overall, our scheme provides a novel perspective of how interfrequency interaction contributes to efficient and dynamic processing of information across the brain.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo , Mamíferos , Animales , Humanos
3.
Neuropsychologia ; 155: 107819, 2021 05 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33684399

RESUMEN

Dyslexia is characterised by poor reading ability. Its aetiology is probably multifactorial, with abnormal visual processing playing an important role. Among adults with normal reading ability, there is a larger representation of central visual field in the primary visual cortex (V1) in those with more efficient visuospatial attention. In this study, we tested the hypothesis that poor reading ability in school-aged children (17 children with dyslexia, 14 control children with normal reading ability) is associated with deficits in visuospatial attention using a visual search task. We corroborated the psychophysical findings with neuroimaging, by measuring the functional size of V1 in response to a central 12° visual stimulus. Consistent with other literature, visual search was impaired and less efficient in the dyslexic children, particularly with more distractor elements in the search array (p = 0.04). We also found atypical interhemispheric asymmetry in functional V1 size in the dyslexia group (p = 0.02). Reading impaired children showed poorer visual search efficiency (p = 0.01), needing more time per unit distractor (higher ms/item). Reading ability was also correlated with V1 size asymmetry (p = 0.03), such that poorer readers showed less left hemisphere bias relative to the right hemisphere. Our findings support the view that dyslexic children have abnormal visuospatial attention and interhemispheric V1 asymmetry, relative to chronological age-matched peers, and that these factors may contribute to inter-individual variation in reading performance in children.


Asunto(s)
Dislexia , Corteza Visual , Adulto , Atención , Niño , Dislexia/diagnóstico por imagen , Humanos , Lectura , Corteza Visual/diagnóstico por imagen , Percepción Visual
4.
J Vis ; 21(1): 13, 2021 01 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33502438

RESUMEN

The features of perceptual surround suppression vary with eccentricity, such that the suppression strength is increased for horizontally oriented stimuli relative to other orientations near the fovea, but is strongest for radially oriented stimuli more peripherally. Perceptual suppression also varies with age, which has been well-studied for central fixation. However, only limited data are available regarding perceptual suppression in older adults for nonfoveal vision, and none of those studies have taken orientation biases of contrast sensitivity into account. Here, we explored the effects of older age on the eccentricity dependency of orientation biases of perceptual suppression. We found increased perceptual suppression in older adults at both 6° and 15° eccentricities relative to younger adults. A main effect of the horizontal orientation bias was found at 6° and a main effect of the radial orientation bias was found at 15° in both groups. In summary, perceptual surround suppression of contrast is stronger for older adults compared with younger adults at 6° and 15° eccentricities, but retinotopic orientation anisotropies are maintained with age. This study provides new insight into parafoveal visual perception in older adults, which may be particularly important to understand the visual experience of those who depend on nonfoveal vision owing to common age-related eye diseases.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Orientación Espacial/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anisotropía , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción Visual , Adulto Joven
5.
Cereb Cortex ; 29(12): 5255-5268, 2019 12 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31220214

RESUMEN

Spike (action potential) responses of most primary visual cortical cells in the macaque are sharply tuned for the orientation of a line or an edge, and neurons preferring similar orientations are clustered together in cortical columns. The preferred stimulus orientation of these columns span the full range of orientations, as observed in recordings of spikes and in classical optical imaging of intrinsic signals. However, when we imaged the putative thalamic input to striate cortical cells that can be seen in imaging of intrinsic signals when they are analyzed on a larger spatial scale, we found that the orientation domain map of the primary visual cortex did not show the same diversity of orientations. This map was dominated by just the one orientation that is most commonly preferred by neurons in the retina and the lateral geniculate nucleus. This supports cortical feature selectivity and columnar architecture being built upon feed-forward signals transmitted from the thalamus in a very limited number of broadly tuned input channels.


Asunto(s)
Neuronas/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Macaca nemestrina , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Vías Visuales/fisiología
6.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30804759

RESUMEN

It has been suggested that the function of the claustrum (CL) may be to orchestrate and integrate the activity of the different cortical areas that are involved in a particular function by boosting the synchronized oscillations that occur between these areas. We propose here a model of how this may be done, thanks to the unique synaptic morphology of the CL and its excitatory and inhibitory connections with most cortical areas. Using serial visual search as an example, we describe how the functional anatomy of the claustral connections can potentially execute the sequential activation of the representations of objects that are being processed serially. We also propose that cross-frequency coupling (CFC) between low frequency signals from CL and higher frequency oscillations in the cortical areas will be an efficient means of CL modulating neural activity across multiple brain regions in synchrony. This model is applicable to the wide range of functions one performs, from simple object recognition to reading and writing, listening to or performing music, etc.


Asunto(s)
Ganglios Basales/fisiología , Corteza Cerebral/citología , Modelos Neurológicos , Vías Nerviosas/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual
7.
Neuropsychologia ; 130: 59-65, 2019 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30794841

RESUMEN

After decades of finding a range of cognitive functions both in visual and phonological domains that correlate with reading performance, there are in recent years attempts to solve the causation versus correlation dilemma in finding a core deficit in developmental dyslexia (DD). Thus, longitudinal studies that aim to predict reading difficulties from studies done in pre-reading years and reading-level matched studies that try to factor out the effect due to lack of reading in DD cohorts, have helped identify two possible candidates to be added to the classical phonological suspect. One is a deficit in visuo-spatial attention that underpins our ability to selectively attend to individual objects in a cluttered world, which is fundamental in being able to identify letters and words in a text such as the one you are reading now. The other is an impairment in synchronised neuronal oscillations that may be crucial in mediating many cortical functions and also communication between brain regions. The latter may be a general deficit affecting many areas of the brain and thus underlie the wide-ranging co-morbidities in DD. However, that neuronal synchrony is a critical mediator in visual attention, brings the two suggestions into one hypothesis of a core deficit that triggers in some young children a great reluctance to read, putting them at a handicap in comparison to other children. This deprives them of the advantage that normal readers have in development of those visual and phonological processes that are needed for reading. This insight into aetiology may help in developing new remediation strategies, specifically aimed at improving visual attention and neuronal synchrony.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Dislexia/psicología , Dislexia/rehabilitación , Percepción Visual , Niño , Preescolar , Dislexia/fisiopatología , Humanos , Estudios Longitudinales , Vías Nerviosas/fisiopatología , Lectura
8.
J Vis ; 18(7): 5, 2018 07 02.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30029269

RESUMEN

Both neurophysiological and psychophysical data provide evidence for orientation biases in nonfoveal vision-specifically, a tendency for a Cartesian horizontal and vertical bias close to fixation, changing to a radial bias with increasing retinal eccentricity. We explore whether the strength of surround suppression of contrast detection also depends on retinotopic location and relative surround configuration (horizontal, vertical, radial, tangential) in parafoveal vision. Three visual-field locations were tested (0°, 225°, and 270°, angle increasing anticlockwise from 0° horizontal axis) at viewing eccentricities of 6° and 15°. Contrast-detection threshold was estimated with and without a surrounding annulus. At 6° eccentricity, horizontally oriented parallel center-surround (C-S) configurations resulted in greater surround suppression compared to vertically oriented parallel center-surround configurations (p = 0.001). At 15° eccentricity, radially oriented parallel center-surround stimuli conferred greater suppression than tangentially oriented stimuli (p = 0.027). Parallel surrounds resulted in greater suppression than orthogonal surrounds at both eccentricities (p < 0.05). At 6° the horizontal center was more susceptible to suppression than a vertical center (p < 0.001) for both parallel and orthogonal surrounds, while at 15° a radial center was more susceptible to suppression (relative to a tangential center), but only if the surround was parallel (p = 0.005). Our data show that orientation anisotropy of surround suppression alters with eccentricity, reflecting a link between suppression strength and visual-field retinotopy.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Anisotropía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación Espacial , Psicofísica , Retina/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
9.
Dyslexia ; 24(1): 33-43, 2018 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29214682

RESUMEN

A major controversy regarding dyslexia is whether any of the many visual and phonological deficits found to be correlated with reading difficulty cause the impairment or result from the reduced amount of reading done by dyslexics. We studied this question by comparing a visual capacity in the left and right visual hemifields in people habitually reading scripts written right-to-left or left-to-right. Selective visual attention is necessary for efficient visual search and also for the sequential recognition of letters in words. Because such attentional allocation during reading depends on the direction in which one is reading, asymmetries in search efficiency may reflect biases arising from the habitual direction of reading. We studied this by examining search performance in three cohorts: (a) left-to-right readers who read English fluently; (b) right-to-left readers fluent in reading Farsi but not any left-to-right script; and (c) bilingual readers fluent in English and in Farsi, Arabic, or Hebrew. Left-to-right readers showed better search performance in the right hemifield and right-to-left readers in the left hemifield, but bilingual readers showed no such asymmetries. Thus, reading experience biases search performance in the direction of reading, which has implications for the cause and effect relationships between reading and cognitive functions.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Dislexia/psicología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Lectura , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Multilingüismo
10.
Physiol Rep ; 5(5)2017 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28270589

RESUMEN

Primate posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is known to be involved in controlling spatial attention. Neurons in one part of the PPC, the lateral intraparietal area (LIP), show enhanced responses to objects at attended locations. Although many are selective for object features, such as the orientation of a visual stimulus, it is not clear how LIP circuits integrate feature-selective information when providing attentional feedback about behaviorally relevant locations to the visual cortex. We studied the relationship between object feature and spatial attention properties of LIP cells in two macaques by measuring the cells' orientation selectivity and the degree of attentional enhancement while performing a delayed match-to-sample task. Monkeys had to match both the location and orientation of two visual gratings presented separately in time. We found a wide range in orientation selectivity and degree of attentional enhancement among LIP neurons. However, cells with significant attentional enhancement had much less orientation selectivity in their response than cells which showed no significant modulation by attention. Additionally, orientation-selective cells showed working memory activity for their preferred orientation, whereas cells showing attentional enhancement also synchronized with local neuronal activity. These results are consistent with models of selective attention incorporating two stages, where an initial feature-selective process guides a second stage of focal spatial attention. We suggest that LIP contributes to both stages, where the first stage involves orientation-selective LIP cells that support working memory of the relevant feature, and the second stage involves attention-enhanced LIP cells that synchronize to provide feedback on spatial priorities.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Animales , Macaca nemestrina , Masculino , Neuronas/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa
11.
Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci ; 58(2): 860-867, 2017 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28159973

RESUMEN

Purpose: The purpose of this study was to study how, in midperipheral vision, aging affects visual processes that interfere with target detection (crowding and surround suppression) and to determine whether the performance on such tasks are related to visuospatial attention as measured by visual search. Methods: We investigated the effect of aging on crowding and suppression in detection of a target in peripheral vision, using different types of flanking stimuli. Both thresholds were also obtained while varying the position of the flanker (placed inside or outside of target, relative to fixation). Crowding thresholds were also estimated with spatial uncertainty (jitter). Additionally, we included a visual search task comprising Gabor stimuli to investigate whether performance is related to top-down attention. Twenty young adults (age, 18-32 years; mean age, 26.1 years; 10 males) and 19 older adults (age, 60-74 years; mean age, 70.3 years; 10 males) participated in the study. Results: Older adults showed more surround suppression than the young (F[1,37] = 4.21; P < 0.05), but crowding was unaffected by age. In the younger group, the position of the flanker influenced the strength of crowding, but not the strength of suppression (F[1,39] = 4.11; P < 0.05). Crowding was not affected by spatial jitter of the stimuli. Neither crowding nor surround suppression was predicted by attentional efficiency measured in the visual search task. There was also no significant correlation between crowding and surround suppression. Conclusions: We show that aging does not affect visual crowding but does increase surround suppression of contrast, suggesting that crowding and surround suppression may be distinct visual phenomena. Furthermore, strengths of crowding and surround suppression did not correlate with each other nor could they be predicted by efficiency of visual search.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Anciano , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto Joven
12.
Trends Neurosci ; 38(8): 475-85, 2015 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26209463

RESUMEN

A common feature of the mammalian striate cortex is the arrangement of 'orientation domains' containing neurons preferring similar stimulus orientations. They are arranged as spokes of a pinwheel that converge at singularities known as 'pinwheel centers'. We propose that a cortical network of feedforward and intracortical lateral connections elaborates a full set of optimum orientations from geniculate inputs that show a bias to stimulus orientation and form a set of two or a small number of 'Cartesian' coordinates. Because each geniculate afferent carries signals only from one eye and its receptive field (RF) is either ON or OFF center, the network constructs also ocular dominance columns and a quasi-segregation of ON and OFF responses across the horizontal extent of the striate cortex.


Asunto(s)
Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Orientación/fisiología , Corteza Visual/anatomía & histología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Vías Visuales/fisiología , Animales , Modelos Neurológicos , Plasticidad Neuronal/fisiología
13.
Eur J Neurosci ; 42(6): 2250-7, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26080026

RESUMEN

Responses of most neurons in the primary visual cortex of mammals are markedly selective for stimulus orientation and their orientation tuning does not vary with changes in stimulus contrast. The basis of such contrast invariance of orientation tuning has been shown to be the higher variability in the response for low-contrast stimuli. Neurons in the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN), which provides the major visual input to the cortex, have also been shown to have higher variability in their response to low-contrast stimuli. Parallel studies have also long established mild degrees of orientation selectivity in LGN and retinal cells. In our study, we show that contrast invariance of orientation tuning is already present in the LGN. In addition, we show that the variability of spike responses of LGN neurons increases at lower stimulus contrasts, especially for non-preferred orientations. We suggest that such contrast- and orientation-sensitive variability not only explains the contrast invariance observed in the LGN but can also underlie the contrast-invariant orientation tuning seen at the level of the primary visual cortex.


Asunto(s)
Sensibilidad de Contraste/fisiología , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Animales , Gatos , Inhibición Neural , Estimulación Luminosa , Vías Visuales/fisiología
14.
Physiol Rep ; 3(4)2015 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25855249

RESUMEN

The primary visual cortex of carnivores and primates shows an orderly progression of domains of neurons that are selective to a particular orientation of visual stimuli such as bars and gratings. We recorded from single-thalamic afferent fibers that terminate in these domains to address the issue whether the orientation sensitivity of these fibers could form the basis of the remarkable orientation selectivity exhibited by most cortical cells. We first performed optical imaging of intrinsic signals to obtain a map of orientation domains on the dorsal aspect of the anaesthetized cat's area 17. After confirming using electrophysiological recordings the orientation preferences of single neurons within one or two domains in each animal, we pharmacologically silenced the cortex to leave only the afferent terminals active. The inactivation of cortical neurons was achieved by the superfusion of either kainic acid or muscimol. Responses of single geniculate afferents were then recorded by the use of high impedance electrodes. We found that the orientation preferences of the afferents matched closely with those of the cells in the orientation domains that they terminated in (Pearson's r = 0.633, n = 22, P = 0.002). This suggests a possible subcortical origin for cortical orientation selectivity.

15.
Neuroimage ; 93 Pt 1: 47-52, 2014 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24583254

RESUMEN

Heavy demands are placed on the brain's attentional capacity when selecting a target item in a cluttered visual scene, or when reading. It is widely accepted that such attentional selection is mediated by top-down signals from higher cortical areas to early visual areas such as the primary visual cortex (V1). Further, it has also been reported that there is considerable variation in the surface area of V1. This variation may impact on either the number or specificity of attentional feedback signals and, thereby, the efficiency of attentional mechanisms. In this study, we investigated whether individual differences between humans performing attention-demanding tasks can be related to the functional area of V1. We found that those with a larger representation in V1 of the central 12° of the visual field as measured using BOLD signals from fMRI were able to perform a serial search task at a faster rate. In line with recent suggestions of the vital role of visuo-spatial attention in reading, the speed of reading showed a strong positive correlation with the speed of visual search, although it showed little correlation with the size of V1. The results support the idea that the functional size of the primary visual cortex is an important determinant of the efficiency of selective spatial attention for simple tasks, and that the attentional processing required for complex tasks like reading are to a large extent determined by other brain areas and inter-areal connections.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Adulto Joven
16.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 7: 811, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24348361

RESUMEN

While phonological impairments are common in developmental dyslexia, there has recently been much debate as to whether there is a causal link between the phonological difficulties and the reading problem. An alternative suggestion has been gaining ground that the core deficit in dyslexia is in visual attentional mechanisms. If so, the visual aetiology may be at any of a number of sites along the afferent magnocellular pathway or in the dorsal cortical stream that are all essential for a visuo-spatial attentional feedback to the primary visual cortex. It has been suggested that the same circuits and pathways of top-down attention used for serial visual search are used for reading. Top-down signals from the dorsal parietal areas to primary visual cortex serially highlight cortical locations representing successive letters in a text before they can be recognized and concatenated into a word. We had shown in non-human primates that the mechanism of such a top-down feedback in a visual attention task uses synchronized neuronal oscillations at the lower end of the gamma frequency range. It is no coincidence that reading graphemes in a text also happens at the low gamma frequencies. The basic proposal here is that each cycle of gamma oscillation focuses an attentional spotlight on the primary visual cortical representation of just one or two letters before sequential recognition of letters and their concatenation into word strings. The timing, period, envelope, amplitude, and phase of the synchronized oscillations modulating the incoming signals in the striate cortex would have a profound influence on the accuracy and speed of reading. Thus, the general temporal sampling difficulties in dyslexic subjects may impact reading not necessarily by causing phonological deficits, but by affecting the spatio-temporal parsing of the visual input within the visual system before these signals are used for letter and word recognition.

17.
J Vis ; 13(8)2013 Jul 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23873673

RESUMEN

Paying attention improves performance, but is this improvement regardless of what we attend to? We explored the differences in performance between attending to a location and attending to a feature when perceiving global motion. Attention was first cued to one of four locations that had coherently moving dots, while the remaining three had randomly moving distracter dots. Participants then viewed a colored display, wherein the color of the coherently moving dots was cued instead of location. In the third task, participants identified the location that had a particular cued direction of motion. Most observers reported reductions of motion threshold in all three tasks compared to when no cue was provided. However, the attentional bias generated by location cues was significantly larger than the bias resulting from feature cues of direction or color. This effect is consistent with the idea that attention is largely controlled by a fronto-parietal network where spatial relations are preferentially processed. On the other hand, color could not be used as a cue to focus attention and integrate motion. This finding suggests that color relies heavily on processing by ventral temporal cortical areas, which may have little control over the global motion areas in the dorsal part of the brain.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Color/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Adulto Joven
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 228(3): 365-76, 2013 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23715717

RESUMEN

When two brief stimuli are presented in rapid succession, our ability to attend and recognize the second stimulus is impaired if our attentional resources are devoted to processing the first. Such inability (termed the "attentional blink" in human studies) arises around 200-500 ms following the onset of the first stimulus. We trained two monkeys on a delayed-match-to-sample task where both the location and orientation of two successively presented grating patches had to be matched. When the delay between the two gratings was varied, monkey's behavioral performance (d') was affected in a way that was analogous to the attentional blink in humans. Furthermore, a subset of neurons in the monkey's lateral intraparietal area, known to be crucial in the control of attention, closely followed the variation in d', even on occasions when d' followed an atypical pattern. Our results provide the first behavioral demonstration of an attentional bottleneck in the macaque of a type similar to the human attentional blink as well as a possible single-neuron correlate of the phenomenon.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Parpadeo Atencional/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Lóbulo Parietal/fisiología , Animales , Macaca nemestrina , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
19.
Clin Exp Optom ; 96(3): 259-66, 2013 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23186138

RESUMEN

In this paper, we review the path taken by signals originating from the short wavelength sensitive cones (S-cones) in Old World and New World primates. Two types of retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) carrying S-cone signals (blue-On and blue-Off cells) project to the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (dLGN) in the thalamus. In all primates, these S-cone signals are relayed through the 'dust-like' (konis in classical Greek) dLGN cells. In New World primates such as common marmoset, these very small cells are known to form distinct and spatially extensive, koniocellular layers. Although in Old World primates, such as macaques, koniocellular layers tend to be very thin, the adjacent parvocellular layers contain distinct koniocellular extensions. It appears that all S-cone signals are relayed through such konio cells, whether they are in the main koniocellular layers or in their colonies within the parvocellular layers of the dLGN. In the primary visual cortex, these signals begin to merge with the signals carried by the other two principal parallel channels, namely the magnocellular and parvocellular channels. This article will also review the possible routes taken by the S-cone signals to reach one of the topographically organised extrastriate visual cortical areas, the middle temporal area (area MT). This area is the major conduit for signals reaching the parietal cortex. Alternative visual inputs to area MT not relayed via the primary visual cortex area (V1) may provide the neurological basis for the phenomenon of 'blindsight' observed in human and non-human primates, who have partial or complete damage to the primary visual cortex. Short wavelength sensitive cone (S-cone) signals to area MT may also play a role in directing visual attention with possible implications for understanding the pathology in dyslexia and some of its treatment options.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Primates/fisiología , Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Animales , Cuerpos Geniculados/fisiología , Humanos , Corteza Visual/fisiología
20.
J Physiol ; 591(1): 339-52, 2013 Jan 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23070701

RESUMEN

We recorded spike activity of single neurones in the middle temporal visual cortical area (MT or V5) of anaesthetised macaque monkeys. We used flashing, stationary spatially circumscribed, cone-isolating and luminance-modulated stimuli of uniform fields to assess the effects of signals originating from the long-, medium- or short- (S) wavelength-sensitive cone classes. Nearly half (41/86) of the tested MT neurones responded reliably to S-cone-isolating stimuli. Response amplitude in the majority of the neurones tested further (19/28) was significantly reduced, though not always completely abolished, during reversible inactivation of visuotopically corresponding regions of the ipsilateral primary visual cortex (striate cortex, area V1). Thus, the present data indicate that signals originating in S-cones reach area MT, either via V1 or via a pathway that does not go through area V1. We did not find a significant difference between the mean latencies of spike responses of MT neurones to signals that bypass V1 and those that do not; the considerable overlap we observed precludes the use of spike-response latency as a criterion to define the routes through which the signals reach MT.


Asunto(s)
Células Fotorreceptoras Retinianas Conos/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Animales , Macaca , Masculino
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA