Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 1.258
Filtrar
1.
Curr Biol ; 34(14): R673-R674, 2024 Jul 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39043136

RESUMEN

Humans regularly engage in efficient communicative conversations, which serve to socially align individuals1. In conversations, we take fast-paced turns using a human-universal structure of deploying and receiving signals which shows consistent timing across cultures2. We report here that chimpanzees also engage in rapid signal-to-signal turn-taking during face-to-face gestural exchanges with a similar average latency between turns to that of human conversation. This correspondence between human and chimpanzee face-to-face communication points to shared underlying rules in communication. These structures could be derived from shared ancestral mechanisms or convergent strategies that enhance coordinated interactions or manage competition for communicative 'space'.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación Animal , Gestos , Lenguaje , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Humanos , Femenino , Masculino
2.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 16556, 2024 07 17.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39019910

RESUMEN

The archaeological record offers insights into our evolutionary past by revealing ancient behaviour through stone and fossil remains. Percussive foraging is suggested to be particularly relevant for the emergence of tool-use in our lineage, yet early hominin percussive behaviours remain largely understudied compared to flaked technology. Stone tool-use of extant primates allows the simultaneous investigation of their artefacts and the associated behaviours. This is important for understanding the development of tool surface modification, and crucial for interpreting damage patterns in the archaeological record. Here, we compare the behaviour and the resulting material record across stone tool-using primates. We investigate the relationship of nut-cracking technique and stone tool modification across chimpanzees, capuchins, and long-tailed macaques by conducting standardized field experiments with comparable raw materials. We show that different techniques likely emerged in response to diverse nut hardness, leading to variation in foraging success across species. Our experiments further demonstrate a correlation between techniques and the intensity of visible percussive damage on the tools. Tools used with more precision and efficiency as demonstrated by macaques, show fewer use wear traces. This suggests that some percussive techniques may be less readily identified in the archaeological record.


Asunto(s)
Arqueología , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Primates , Macaca , Cebus , Fósiles
3.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 17135, 2024 07 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39054330

RESUMEN

Nonhuman great apes have been claimed to be unable to learn human words due to a lack of the necessary neural circuitry. We recovered original footage of two enculturated chimpanzees uttering the word "mama" and subjected recordings to phonetic analysis. Our analyses demonstrate that chimpanzees are capable of syllabic production, achieving consonant-to-vowel phonetic contrasts via the simultaneous recruitment and coupling of voice, jaw and lips. In an online experiment, human listeners naive to the recordings' origins reliably perceived chimpanzee utterances as syllabic utterances, primarily as "ma-ma", among foil syllables. Our findings demonstrate that in the absence of direct data-driven examination, great ape vocal production capacities have been underestimated. Chimpanzees possess the neural building blocks necessary for speech.


Asunto(s)
Pan troglodytes , Habla , Vocalización Animal , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Animales , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Femenino , Fonética
4.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 17579, 2024 07 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39080416

RESUMEN

Humans modify their environment to grant or prevent others' access to valuable resources, for example by using locks. We tested whether sanctuary-living chimpanzees (N = 10) would flexibly modify their environment to either allow or deny a dominant conspecific access to a shared food source by giving them the option to change a food reward's pathway prior to releasing it. The food could end up in one of two locations: one was accessible to both the subject and a dominant conspecific, the other one was only accessible to the subject. We further manipulated the extent of inhibitory control needed for modifying the pathway by varying the subjects' starting position. Our subjects reoriented the pathway competitively to monopolize food but changed the pathway less often in trials with high inhibitory demands. We further show how inhibitory task demands in a social context influence chimpanzees' future planning. Our results show that chimpanzees will strategically manipulate their environment to maximize their own and deny a dominant conspecific access to food.


Asunto(s)
Alimentos , Pan troglodytes , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino , Recompensa , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Conducta Animal , Conducta Social
5.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 15896, 2024 07 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38987277

RESUMEN

Humans categorize body parts, reflecting our knowledge about bodies, and this could be useful in higher-level activities involving bodies. We tested whether humans' closest living relatives-chimpanzees-have the same ability using touchscreen tasks, focusing on the major parts: heads, torsos, arms, and legs. Six chimpanzees were trained to perform a body part matching-to-sample task using sets of pictures of chimpanzee bodies, where in each trial, the sample and choice pictures were the same. Five passed the training and received the test sessions, where three trial types were mixed: trained same-individual picture pairs; novel same-individual picture pairs; and novel different-individual picture pairs. All participants performed better than the chance level in all conditions and for all body parts. Further analyses showed differences in performance when the samples were different body parts. For example, the results revealed better performances for heads and torsos than arms and legs in "novel different-individual pairs". The study showed that chimpanzees can visually match and categorize body parts in this experiment setting, even across different chimpanzees' bodies, suggesting potential biological understanding. Different performances for body parts suggested a deviated categorization from humans. We hope this study will inspire future research on the evolution of body perception.


Asunto(s)
Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Masculino , Femenino
6.
Cereb Cortex ; 34(6)2024 Jun 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38869374

RESUMEN

The central sulcus divides the primary motor and somatosensory cortices in many anthropoid primate brains. Differences exist in the surface area and depth of the central sulcus along the dorso-ventral plane in great apes and humans compared to other primate species. Within hominid species, there are variations in the depth and aspect of their hand motor area, or knob, within the precentral gyrus. In this study, we used post-image analyses on magnetic resonance images to characterize the central sulcus shape of humans, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus and Pongo abelii). Using these data, we examined the morphological variability of central sulcus in hominids, focusing on the hand region, a significant change in human evolution. We show that the central sulcus shape differs between great ape species, but all show similar variations in the location of their hand knob. However, the prevalence of the knob location along the dorso-ventral plane and lateralization differs between species and the presence of a second ventral motor knob seems to be unique to humans. Humans and orangutans exhibit the most similar and complex central sulcus shapes. However, their similarities may reflect divergent evolutionary processes related to selection for different positional and habitual locomotor functions.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Gorilla gorilla , Hominidae , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Corteza Motora , Pan troglodytes , Filogenia , Animales , Humanos , Masculino , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Gorilla gorilla/fisiología , Femenino , Corteza Motora/anatomía & histología , Corteza Motora/fisiología , Corteza Motora/diagnóstico por imagen , Hominidae/anatomía & histología , Hominidae/fisiología , Adulto , Mano/fisiología , Mano/anatomía & histología , Adulto Joven , Pongo pygmaeus/anatomía & histología , Pongo pygmaeus/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie , Pongo abelii/anatomía & histología , Pongo abelii/fisiología
7.
Biol Lett ; 20(6): 20240051, 2024 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38863345

RESUMEN

When chimpanzees search for hidden food, do they realize that their guesses may not be correct? We applied a post-decision wagering paradigm to a simple two-cup search task, varying whether we gave participants visual access to the baiting and then asking after they had chosen one of the cups whether they would prefer a smaller but certain reward instead of their original choice (experiment 1). Results showed that chimpanzees were more likely to accept the smaller reward in occluded than visible conditions. Experiment 2 found the same effect when we blocked visual access but manipulated the number of hiding locations for the food piece, showing that the effect is not owing to representation type. Experiments 3 and 4 showed that when given information about the contents of the unchosen cup, chimpanzees were able to flexibly update their choice behaviour accordingly. These results suggest that language is not a pre-requisite to solving the disjunctive syllogism and provides a valuable contribution to the debate on logical reasoning in non-human animals.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Recompensa
8.
Sci Rep ; 14(1): 12155, 2024 05 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38802445

RESUMEN

Differences in the tool use of non-human primates and humans are subject of ongoing debate. In humans, representations of object functions underpin efficient tool use. Yet, representations of object functions can lead to functional fixedness, which describes the fixation on a familiar tool function leading to less efficient problem solving when the problem requires using the tool for a new function. In the current study, we examined whether chimpanzees exhibit functional fixedness. After solving a problem with a tool, chimpanzees were less efficient in solving another problem which required using the same tool with a different function compared to a control group. This fixation effect was still apparent after a period of nine months and when chimpanzees had learned about the function of a tool by observation of a conspecific. These results suggest that functional fixedness in our closest living relatives likely exists and cast doubt on the notion that stable function representations are uniquely human.


Asunto(s)
Pan troglodytes , Solución de Problemas , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Masculino , Femenino , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta/fisiología , Aprendizaje , Humanos
9.
Curr Biol ; 34(9): R353-R355, 2024 05 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38714164

RESUMEN

A new paper shows that rates of aggression are higher, and rates of coalition formation are lower, among male bonobos than among male chimpanzees. These findings are noteworthy because they challenge the view that female bonobos' preferences for less aggressive males favored a reduction in male aggression and an increase in social tolerance.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Conducta Animal , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan paniscus/psicología , Pan paniscus/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Conducta Animal/fisiología , Conducta Social
10.
J Acoust Soc Am ; 155(5): 3206-3212, 2024 May 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38738937

RESUMEN

Modern humans and chimpanzees share a common ancestor on the phylogenetic tree, yet chimpanzees do not spontaneously produce speech or speech sounds. The lab exercise presented in this paper was developed for undergraduate students in a course entitled "What's Special About Human Speech?" The exercise is based on acoustic analyses of the words "cup" and "papa" as spoken by Viki, a home-raised, speech-trained chimpanzee, as well as the words spoken by a human. The analyses allow students to relate differences in articulation and vocal abilities between Viki and humans to the known anatomical differences in their vocal systems. Anatomical and articulation differences between humans and Viki include (1) potential tongue movements, (2) presence or absence of laryngeal air sacs, (3) presence or absence of vocal membranes, and (4) exhalation vs inhalation during production.


Asunto(s)
Pan troglodytes , Acústica del Lenguaje , Habla , Humanos , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , Lengua/fisiología , Lengua/anatomía & histología , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Especificidad de la Especie , Medición de la Producción del Habla , Laringe/fisiología , Laringe/anatomía & histología , Fonética
11.
Primates ; 65(4): 243-255, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38816634

RESUMEN

Although chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and bonobos (Pan paniscus) share a multi-male/multi-female societal organization and form male-philopatric groups, disparities in terms of male aggression and stability of temporary parties are thought to exist among them. However, existing research in bonobos has mainly focused on the high social status, prolonged receptivity, and characteristic sexual behaviors of females, leaving the behaviors of males understudied. Moreover, prior comparative studies on Pan suffer from methodological inconsistencies. This study addresses these gaps by employing a uniform observation method to explore party attendance and aggressive interactions among male bonobos in Wamba and male chimpanzees in Kalinzu. Unlike male chimpanzees, which exhibit dispersion in the absence of receptive females in the group, male bonobos showed a lesser degree of such dispersion. Although the overall frequency of aggressive interactions per observation unit did not significantly differ between the two species, the nature of these interactions varied. Notably, severe aggressive behaviors such as physical confrontations among adult males were absent in bonobos, with most aggression occurring between the sons of the two highest-ranking females. Additionally, in bonobos, females actively engaged in polyadic aggressive behavior as aggressors, while all instances of coalitionary aggression in chimpanzees originated from male aggressors. These findings underscore the substantial impact of female behaviors on the observed distinctions in male aggressive interactions between the two species.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Conducta Social , Animales , Masculino , Pan paniscus/fisiología , Pan paniscus/psicología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Femenino , República Democrática del Congo , Uganda
12.
Am J Primatol ; 86(8): e23633, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38775638

RESUMEN

Heart rate is a crucial vital sign and a valuable indicator for assessing the physical and psychological condition of a target animal. Heart rate contributes to (1) fundamental information for cognitive research, (2) an indicator of psychological and physical stress, and (3) improving the animal welfare of captive animals, especially in nonhuman primate studies. Heart rate has been measured using a contact-type device; however, the device burdens the target animals and that there are risks associated with anesthesia during installation. This study explores the application of heartbeat measurement techniques using millimeter-wave radar, primarily developed for humans, as a remote and noninvasive method for measuring the heart rate of nonhuman primates. Through a measurement test conducted on two chimpanzees, we observed a remarkable correspondence between the peak frequency spectrum of heart rate estimated using millimeter-wave radar and the mean value obtained from electrocardiograph data, thereby validating the accuracy of the method. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first demonstration of the precise measurement of great apes' heart rate using millimeter-wave radar technology. Compared to heart rate measurement using video analysis, the method using millimeter-wave radar has the advantage that it is less susceptible to weather and lighting conditions and that measurement techniques for multiple individuals have been developed for human subjects, while its disadvantage is that validation of measurement from long distances has not been completed. Another disadvantage common to both methods is that measurement becomes difficult when the movement of the target individual is large. The possibility of noncontact measurement of heart rate in wild and captive primates will undoubtedly open up a new research area while taking animal welfare into consideration.


Asunto(s)
Frecuencia Cardíaca , Pan troglodytes , Radar , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Electrocardiografía/veterinaria , Electrocardiografía/instrumentación
13.
PLoS Biol ; 22(5): e3002609, 2024 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38713644

RESUMEN

Tool use is considered a driving force behind the evolution of brain expansion and prolonged juvenile dependency in the hominin lineage. However, it remains rare across animals, possibly due to inherent constraints related to manual dexterity and cognitive abilities. In our study, we investigated the ontogeny of tool use in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), a species known for its extensive and flexible tool use behavior. We observed 70 wild chimpanzees across all ages and analyzed 1,460 stick use events filmed in the Taï National Park, Côte d'Ivoire during the chimpanzee attempts to retrieve high-nutrient, but difficult-to-access, foods. We found that chimpanzees increasingly utilized hand grips employing more than 1 independent digit as they matured. Such hand grips emerged at the age of 2, became predominant and fully functional at the age of 6, and ubiquitous at the age of 15, enhancing task accuracy. Adults adjusted their hand grip based on the specific task at hand, favoring power grips for pounding actions and intermediate grips that combine power and precision, for others. Highly protracted development of suitable actions to acquire hidden (i.e., larvae) compared to non-hidden (i.e., nut kernel) food was evident, with adult skill levels achieved only after 15 years, suggesting a pronounced cognitive learning component to task success. The prolonged time required for cognitive assimilation compared to neuromotor control points to selection pressure favoring the retention of learning capacities into adulthood.


Asunto(s)
Fuerza de la Mano , Pan troglodytes , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Fuerza de la Mano/fisiología , Côte d'Ivoire , Cognición/fisiología , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología
14.
Primates ; 65(4): 217-222, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38668987

RESUMEN

Heart rate measurements can be useful for the monitoring of both physical and mental condition in humans and nonhuman animals. Yet, information about heart rates in chimpanzees, who are phylogenetically the closest species to humans, is scarce. Existing data on chimpanzee heart rates have mainly been collected from chimpanzees under anesthesia. To address this issue, we conducted electrocardiogram recordings in captive chimpanzees under normal conditions without anesthesia based on positive reinforcement training. We obtained a total of 771 recordings from 35 individuals (22 males and 13 females, 14-53 years old) with no cardiac problems. The females had a higher heart rate than the male chimpanzees, and heart rate decreased as a function of age. In addition, heart rate was lower in the morning and increased during the day. Overall, the mean heart rate of adult males was 86.5 beats/min, and that of female chimpanzees 106.4 beats/min. Our data could serve as a reference point for future research and health-based monitoring of chimpanzee heart rates.


Asunto(s)
Frecuencia Cardíaca , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Masculino , Femenino , Electrocardiografía/veterinaria , Anestesia/veterinaria , Factores Sexuales
15.
Primates ; 65(4): 209-215, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38687456

RESUMEN

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are categorized as Endangered by the International Union for Conservation of Nature, and habitat loss due to conversion of land for agriculture is one of the major threats to wild populations of this species. This challenging scenario can lead to negative human-chimpanzee interactions, including crop feeding. Chimpanzees consume crops across their geographical range, although little is known about this behavior in savanna habitats. Here we provide new evidence of crop feeding by savanna chimpanzees. We conducted our observations at Dindefelo, a community nature reserve in southeastern Senegal. The chimpanzees were observed to feed on mango (Mangifera indica) and also on baobab (Adansonia digitata), a wild species considered a crop by local people when found in and around villages. Although local people use the fruits of these species for food and income, they tolerated crop-feeding events until recently. In 2023, a case of harassment of a crop-feeding chimpanzee in a mango orchard was witnessed, and four days later a chimpanzee corpse was found at the same place. We conclude that habitat conversion into agricultural fields, uncontrolled bush fires and extraction of wild fruits are the important factors influencing crop-feeding events at Dindefelo. Our findings highlight the need to better understand human-chimpanzee interactions in the anthropogenic landscape of Dindefelo to help mitigate negative attitudes and behaviors towards chimpanzees.


Asunto(s)
Conservación de los Recursos Naturales , Conducta Alimentaria , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Senegal , Mangifera , Pradera , Productos Agrícolas , Femenino , Masculino
16.
Curr Biol ; 34(8): 1780-1785.e4, 2024 04 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38614078

RESUMEN

Researchers investigating the evolution of human aggression look to our closest living relatives, bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes), as valuable sources of comparative data.1,2 Males in the two species exhibit contrasting patterns: male chimpanzees sexually coerce females3,4,5,6,7,8 and sometimes kill conspecifics,9,10,11,12 whereas male bonobos exhibit less sexual coercion13,14 and no reported killing.13 Among the various attempts to explain these species differences, the self-domestication hypothesis proposes negative fitness consequences of male aggression in bonobos.2,15,16 Nonetheless, the extent to which these species differ in overall rates of aggression remains unclear due to insufficiently comparable observation methods.17,18,19,20,21,22,23 We used 14 community-years of focal follow data-the gold standard for observational studies24-to compare rates of male aggression in 3 bonobo communities at the Kokolopori Bonobo Reserve, Democratic Republic of Congo, and 2 chimpanzee communities at Gombe National Park, Tanzania. As expected, given that females commonly outrank males, we found that bonobos exhibited lower rates of male-female aggression and higher rates of female-male aggression than chimpanzees. Surprisingly, we found higher rates of male-male aggression among bonobos than chimpanzees even when limiting analyses to contact aggression. In both species, more aggressive males obtained higher mating success. Although our findings indicate that the frequency of male-male aggression does not parallel species difference in its intensity, they support the view that contrary to male chimpanzees, whose reproductive success depends on strong coalitions, male bonobos have more individualistic reproductive strategies.25.


Asunto(s)
Agresión , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan paniscus/psicología , Pan paniscus/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Masculino , República Democrática del Congo , Tanzanía , Femenino , Especificidad de la Especie , Conducta Sexual Animal/fisiología
17.
Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc ; 99(4): 1556-1575, 2024 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38597291

RESUMEN

Empathy is a complex, multi-dimensional capacity that facilitates the sharing and understanding of others' emotions. As our closest living relatives, bonobos (Pan paniscus) and chimpanzees (P. troglodytes) provide an opportunity to explore the origins of hominin social cognition, including empathy. Despite certain assumptions that bonobos and chimpanzees may differ empathically, these species appear to overlap considerably in certain socio-emotional responses related to empathy. However, few studies have systematically tested for species variation in Pan empathic or socio-emotional tendencies. To address this, we synthesise the growing literature on Pan empathy to inform our understanding of the selection pressures that may underlie the evolution of hominin empathy, and its expression in our last common ancestor. As bonobos and chimpanzees show overlaps in their expression of complex socio-emotional phenomena such as empathy, we propose that group comparisons may be as or more meaningful than species comparisons when it comes to understanding the evolutionary pressures for such behaviour. Furthermore, key differences, such as how humans and Pan communicate, appear to distinguish how we experience empathy compared to our closest living relatives.


Asunto(s)
Empatía , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan paniscus/psicología , Pan paniscus/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Humanos , Evolución Biológica , Conducta Social , Especificidad de la Especie
18.
Behav Res Methods ; 56(7): 6520-6537, 2024 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38438657

RESUMEN

Parsing signals from noise is a general problem for signallers and recipients, and for researchers studying communicative systems. Substantial efforts have been invested in comparing how other species encode information and meaning, and how signalling is structured. However, research depends on identifying and discriminating signals that represent meaningful units of analysis. Early approaches to defining signal repertoires applied top-down approaches, classifying cases into predefined signal types. Recently, more labour-intensive methods have taken a bottom-up approach describing detailed features of each signal and clustering cases based on patterns of similarity in multi-dimensional feature-space that were previously undetectable. Nevertheless, it remains essential to assess whether the resulting repertoires are composed of relevant units from the perspective of the species using them, and redefining repertoires when additional data become available. In this paper we provide a framework that takes data from the largest set of wild chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) gestures currently available, splitting gesture types at a fine scale based on modifying features of gesture expression using latent class analysis (a model-based cluster detection algorithm for categorical variables), and then determining whether this splitting process reduces uncertainty about the goal or community of the gesture. Our method allows different features of interest to be incorporated into the splitting process, providing substantial future flexibility across, for example, species, populations, and levels of signal granularity. Doing so, we provide a powerful tool allowing researchers interested in gestural communication to establish repertoires of relevant units for subsequent analyses within and between systems of communication.


Asunto(s)
Comunicación Animal , Gestos , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Algoritmos , Análisis por Conglomerados , Masculino , Femenino
19.
Am J Biol Anthropol ; 184(3): e24931, 2024 07.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38491922

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVES: Integration reflects the level of coordinated variation of the phenotype. The integration of postcranial elements can be studied from a functional perspective, especially with regards to locomotion. This study investigates the link between locomotion, femoral structural properties, and femur-pelvis complex morphology. MATERIALS AND METHODS: We measured (1) morphological integration between femoral and pelvic morphologies using geometric morphometrics, and (2) covariation between femoral/pelvic morphologies and femoral diaphyseal cross-sectional properties, which we defined as morpho-structural integration. Morphological and morpho-structural integration patterns were measured among humans (n = 19), chimpanzees and bonobos (n = 16), and baboons (n = 14), whose locomotion are distinct. RESULTS: Baboons show the highest magnitude of morphological integration and the lowest of morpho-structural integration. Chimpanzees and bonobos show intermediate magnitude of morphological and morpho-structural integration. Yet, body size seems to have a considerable influence on both integration patterns, limiting the interpretations. Finally, humans present the lowest morphological integration and the highest morpho-structural integration between femoral morphology and structural properties but not between pelvic morphology and femur. DISCUSSION: Morphological and morpho-structural integration depict distinct strategies among the samples. A strong morphological integration among baboon's femur-pelvis module might highlight evidence for long-term adaptation to quadrupedalism. In humans, it is likely that distinct selective pressures associated with the respective function of the pelvis and the femur tend to decrease morphological integration. Conversely, high mechanical loading on the hindlimbs during bipedal locomotion might result in specific combination of structural and morphological features within the femur.


Asunto(s)
Fémur , Locomoción , Animales , Fémur/anatomía & histología , Fémur/fisiología , Femenino , Masculino , Humanos , Locomoción/fisiología , Pelvis/anatomía & histología , Pelvis/fisiología , Pan paniscus/fisiología , Pan paniscus/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Antropología Física , Huesos Pélvicos/anatomía & histología , Huesos Pélvicos/fisiología , Adulto , Papio/fisiología , Papio/anatomía & histología
20.
J Anat ; 245(1): 156-180, 2024 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38381116

RESUMEN

Extant great apes are characterized by a wide range of locomotor, postural and manipulative behaviours that each require the limbs to be used in different ways. In addition to external bone morphology, comparative investigation of trabecular bone, which (re-)models to reflect loads incurred during life, can provide novel insights into bone functional adaptation. Here, we use canonical holistic morphometric analysis (cHMA) to analyse the trabecular morphology in the distal femoral epiphysis of Homo sapiens (n = 26), Gorilla gorilla (n = 14), Pan troglodytes (n = 15) and Pongo sp. (n = 9). We test two predictions: (1) that differing locomotor behaviours will be reflected in differing trabecular architecture of the distal femur across Homo, Pan, Gorilla and Pongo; (2) that trabecular architecture will significantly differ between male and female Gorilla due to their different levels of arboreality but not between male and female Pan or Homo based on previous studies of locomotor behaviours. Results indicate that trabecular architecture differs among extant great apes based on their locomotor repertoires. The relative bone volume and degree of anisotropy patterns found reflect habitual use of extended knee postures during bipedalism in Homo, and habitual use of flexed knee posture during terrestrial and arboreal locomotion in Pan and Gorilla. Trabecular architecture in Pongo is consistent with a highly mobile knee joint that may vary in posture from extension to full flexion. Within Gorilla, trabecular architecture suggests a different loading of knee in extension/flexion between females and males, but no sex differences were found in Pan or Homo, supporting our predictions. Inter- and intra-specific variation in trabecular architecture of distal femur provides a comparative context to interpret knee postures and, in turn, locomotor behaviours in fossil hominins.


Asunto(s)
Hueso Esponjoso , Fémur , Hominidae , Animales , Masculino , Femenino , Fémur/anatomía & histología , Hominidae/anatomía & histología , Hominidae/fisiología , Humanos , Hueso Esponjoso/anatomía & histología , Locomoción/fisiología , Gorilla gorilla/anatomía & histología , Gorilla gorilla/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA