Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 58
Filtrar
1.
Brain Sci ; 14(4)2024 Mar 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38671988

RESUMEN

Determination of language hemispheric dominance (HD) in patients undergoing evaluation for epilepsy surgery has traditionally relied on the sodium amobarbital (Wada) test. The emergence of non-invasive methods for determining language laterality has increasingly shown to be a viable alternative. In this study, we assessed the efficacy of transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) and magnetoencephalography (MEG), compared to the Wada test, in determining language HD in a sample of 12 patients. TMS-induced speech errors were classified as speech arrest, semantic, or performance errors, and the HD was based on the total number of errors in each hemisphere with equal weighting of all errors (classic) and with a higher weighting of speech arrests and semantic errors (weighted). Using MEG, HD for language was based on the spatial extent of long-latency activity sources localized to receptive language regions. Based on the classic and weighted language laterality index (LI) in 12 patients, TMS was concordant with the Wada in 58.33% and 66.67% of patients, respectively. In eight patients, MEG language mapping was deemed conclusive, with a concordance rate of 75% with the Wada test. Our results indicate that TMS and MEG have moderate and strong agreement, respectively, with the Wada test, suggesting they could be used as non-invasive substitutes.

2.
J Neurosurg Pediatr ; 34(1): 19-29, 2024 Jul 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38669716

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: Language dominance in the developing brain can vary widely across anatomical and pathological conditions as well as age groups. Repetitive navigated transcranial magnetic stimulation (rnTMS) has been applied to calculate the hemispheric dominance ratio (HDR) in adults. In this study, the authors aimed to assess the feasibility of using rnTMS to identify language lateralization in a pediatric neurosurgical cohort and to correlate the preoperative rnTMS findings with the postoperative language outcome. METHODS: A consecutive prospectively collected cohort of 19 children with language-associated lesions underwent bihemispheric rnTMS mapping prior to surgery (100 stimulation sites on each hemisphere). In addition to feasibility and adverse effects, the HDR (ratio of the left hemisphere to right hemisphere error rate) was calculated. The anatomical surgical site and postoperative language outcome at 3 months after surgery were assessed according to clinical documentation. RESULTS: Repetitive nTMS mapping was feasible in all 19 children (mean age 12.5 years, range 4-17 years; 16 left-sided lesions) without any relevant adverse events. Thirteen children (68%) showed left hemispheric dominance (HDR > 1.1), and 2 children (11%) showed right hemispheric dominance (HDR < 0.9). In 4 children (21%), the bihemispheric error rates were nearly the same (HDR ≥ 0.9 and ≤ 1.1). Sixteen children underwent surgery (14 tumor/lesion resections and 2 hemispherotomies) and 3 patients continued conservative therapy. After surgery, 4 patients (25%) showed an improvement in language function, 10 (63%) presented with stable language function, and 2 (12.5%) experienced deterioration in language function. Of the 6 patients with right hemispheric language involvement, 4 (80%) had glial tumors, 1 (20%) had focal cortical dysplasia, and 1 (20%) experienced hypoxic brain injury. Children with right hemispheric language involvement (HDR ≤ 1.1) did not show any language deterioration postoperatively. CONCLUSIONS: Bihemispheric rnTMS language mapping as a noninvasive mapping technique to assess lateralization of language function in the pediatric neurosurgical population is safe and feasible. Why relevant right hemispheric language function (HDR ≤ 1.1) was associated with postoperative unaltered language function needs to be validated in future studies. Bihemispheric rnTMS language mapping strengthens risk-benefit considerations prior to pediatric tumor/epilepsy surgery in language-associated areas.


Asunto(s)
Neuronavegación , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal , Humanos , Niño , Masculino , Femenino , Adolescente , Preescolar , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal/métodos , Neuronavegación/métodos , Estudios Prospectivos , Lenguaje , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos , Neoplasias Encefálicas/cirugía , Estudios de Factibilidad , Procedimientos Neuroquirúrgicos/métodos , Resultado del Tratamiento
3.
Eur J Neurol ; 30(6): 1555-1556, 2023 06.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36880650

RESUMEN

In this issue of European Journal of Neurology, Robinson et al. present a novel study on primary progressive apraxia of speech. The authors describe different clinicopathological profiles in patients with left-dominant, right-dominant, and bilateral atrophy of the supplementary motor area and lateral premotor cortex. This commentary discusses the importance of this evidence for understanding individual differences among these patients, distinguishing them from those with nonfluent variant primary progressive aphasia, and analyzing the relations between motor speech deficits and underlying pathology.


Asunto(s)
Afasia Progresiva Primaria , Apraxias , Humanos , Afasia Progresiva Primaria/diagnóstico , Encéfalo/patología , Habla , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Apraxias/diagnóstico
4.
J Neurol ; 270(4): 2174-2183, 2023 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36633670

RESUMEN

The incidence and characteristics of acute vestibular symptoms, responsible structures, and lateralization of the causative lesions in supratentorial stroke remain unknown. This study aimed to determine the incidence, clinical features, and anatomical correlation of acute vestibular symptoms in supratentorial stroke. We performed a prospective, multicenter, observational study that had recruited patients with supratentorial stroke from the neurology clinics of referral-based four university hospitals in Korea. All patients received a constructed neuro-otological evaluations, and neuroimaging. We analyzed the incidence of acute vestibular symptoms, abnormal ocular motor and vestibular function tests, and stroke lesions. Of 1301 patients with supratentorial stroke, 48 (3.7%) presented with acute vestibular symptoms, and 13 of them (1%) had the vestibular symptoms in isolation. In patients with acute vestibular symptoms, abnormal findings included spontaneous nystagmus (5%), impaired horizontal smooth pursuit (41%), and abnormal tilt of the subjective visual vertical (SVV) (20%). Video head impulse and caloric tests were normal in all the patients. There was no clear correlation between acute vestibular symptoms and involvement of specific vestibular cortex. In patients with unilateral stroke, there was also no lateralization of the causative lesions of acute vestibular symptoms (left vs. right; 52 vs. 48%), even in patients with vertigo (left vs. right; 58 vs. 42%). This study demonstrates that the incidence of acute vestibular symptoms in supratentorial stroke is 3.7%, with being isolated in 1%. The widespread lesions responsible for acute vestibular symptoms implicate diffuse multisensory cortical-subcortical networks in the cerebral hemispheres without a lateralization.


Asunto(s)
Nistagmo Patológico , Accidente Cerebrovascular , Humanos , Incidencia , Estudios Prospectivos , Vértigo/diagnóstico , Accidente Cerebrovascular/complicaciones , Accidente Cerebrovascular/diagnóstico por imagen , Accidente Cerebrovascular/epidemiología , Nistagmo Patológico/diagnóstico por imagen , Nistagmo Patológico/epidemiología , Nistagmo Patológico/etiología
5.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 2022 Dec 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36576683

RESUMEN

Many functions of the human brain are organized asymmetrically and are subject to strong population biases. Some tasks, like speaking and making complex hand movements, exhibit left hemispheric dominance, whereas others, such as spatial processing and recognizing faces, favor the right hemisphere. While pattern of preference implies the existence of a stereotypical way of distributing functions between the hemispheres, an ever-increasing body of evidence indicates that not everyone follows this pattern of hemispheric functional segregation. On the contrary, the review conducted in this article shows that departures from the standard hemispheric division of labor are routinely observed and assume many distinct forms, each having a different prevalence rate. One of the key challenges in human neuroscience is to model this variability. By integrating well-established and recently emerged ideas about the mechanisms that underlie functional lateralization, the current article proposes a general mechanistic model that explains the observed distribution of segregation phenotypes and generates new testable hypotheses.

6.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 14: 932318, 2022.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35813955

RESUMEN

Background: There was a reorganization of the brain network after stroke. Some studies have compared the characteristics of activation or functional connectivity (FC) of cortical and subcortical regions between the dominant and non-dominant hemisphere stroke. Objectives: To analyze hemispheric dominance differences in task-state motor network properties in subacute stroke by functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS). Materials and Methods: Patients with first ischemic stroke in the basal ganglia within 1-3 months after onset and age- and sex-matched right-handed healthy subjects (HS) were enrolled. fNIRS with 29 channels was used to detect the oxyhemoglobin concentration changes when performing the hand grasping task. Activation patterns of motor cortex and two macroscale and two mesoscale brain network indicators based on graph theory were compared between dominant and non-dominant hemisphere stroke. Results: We enrolled 17 subjects in each of left hemisphere stroke (LHS), right hemisphere stroke (RHS), and HS groups. Both patient groups showed bilateral activation. The average weighted clustering coefficient and global efficiency of patients were lower than those of healthy people, and the inter-density was higher than that of the HS group, but the significance was different between LHS and RHS groups. The intra-density changes in the RHS group were opposite to those in the LHS group. The correlation between mesoscale indicators and motor function differed between dominant and non-dominant hemisphere stroke. Conclusion: The changes in macroscale cortical network indicators were similar between the two patient groups, while those of the mesoscale indicators were different. The mesoscale brain network characteristics were affected by the severity of dysfunction to varying degrees in the LHS and RHS patients.

7.
Exp Brain Res ; 240(9): 2311-2326, 2022 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35876852

RESUMEN

The evidence for the hemispheric specialization of motor planning reveals several inconsistencies between the left-lateralized hypothesis and a distributed system across the hemispheres. We compared participants with left hemiplegic cerebral palsy (HCP) to right-handed control subjects in this study's first experiment by inviting them to perform a motor planning task. Participants were required to release the start button, grasp a hexagon, and rotate it according to the instructions. In the second experiment, we compared left-HCP subjects with right-HCP subjects inviting them to perform the same task (we used the data for left-HCP subjects from the first experiment). P2 amplitude, as well as planning time, grasping time, releasing time, and initial grip selection planning patterns, were used as outcome measures in both experiments. The first experiment revealed that controls acted more quickly and chose more effective planning patterns. Also, the P2 amplitude was smaller in left-HCP subjects than in control subjects. No significant group effect was observed in the second experiment for any movement-related measure or P2. At the neural level, however, there was an interaction between 'region' and 'group,' indicating the distinction between the two groups in the right region. The results are discussed in terms of motor planning's hemispheric distribution and individual differences in the HCP group.


Asunto(s)
Parálisis Cerebral , Parálisis Cerebral/complicaciones , Potenciales Evocados , Lateralidad Funcional , Fuerza de la Mano , Hemiplejía/etiología , Humanos , Desempeño Psicomotor
8.
Front Neurol ; 12: 789487, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34956067

RESUMEN

This study aimed to investigate the disparity in locomotor and spatial memory deficits caused by left- or right-sided unilateral vestibular deafferentation (UVD) using a mouse model of unilateral labyrinthectomy (UL) and to examine the effects of galvanic vestibular stimulation (GVS) on the deficits over 14 days. Five experimental groups were established: the left-sided and right-sided UL (Lt.-UL and Rt.-UL) groups, left-sided and right-sided UL with bipolar GVS with the cathode on the lesion side (Lt.-GVS and Rt.-GVS) groups, and a control group with sham surgery. We assessed the locomotor and cognitive-behavioral functions using the open field (OF), Y maze, and Morris water maze (MWM) tests before (baseline) and 3, 7, and 14 days after surgical UL in each group. On postoperative day (POD) 3, locomotion and spatial working memory were more impaired in the Lt.-UL group compared with the Rt.-UL group (p < 0.01, Tamhane test). On POD 7, there was a substantial difference between the groups; the locomotion and spatial navigation of the Lt.-UL group recovered significantly more slowly compared with those of the Rt.-UL group. Although the differences in the short-term spatial cognition and motor coordination were resolved by POD 14, the long-term spatial navigation deficits assessed by the MWM were significantly worse in the Lt.-UL group compared with the Rt.-UL group. GVS intervention accelerated the vestibular compensation in both the Lt.-GVS and Rt.-GVS groups in terms of improvement of locomotion and spatial cognition. The current data imply that right- and left-sided UVD impair spatial cognition and locomotion differently and result in different compensatory patterns. Sequential bipolar GVS when the cathode (stimulating) was assigned to the lesion side accelerated recovery for UVD-induced spatial cognition, which may have implications for managing the patients with spatial cognitive impairment, especially that induced by unilateral peripheral vestibular damage on the dominant side.

9.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 228: 109076, 2021 11 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34600250

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: People with substance use disorders (SUDs) usually experience emotion dysregulation, which may be a consequence of or a risk factor for the development and maintenance of substance misuse. Despite growing evidence on emotion dysregulation among people with SUDs, relatively few studies have explored emotion dysregulation in heroin use disorder (HUD) patients. METHODS: Using event-related potentials (ERP), we compared the emotion regulation ability of 33 HUD patients and 30 healthy controls according to their average electroencephalogram amplitudes of the late positive potential (LPP) component in 400-1000 ms and 1000-2000 ms time windows, while viewing neutral and unpleasant emotional pictures, and using emotion regulation strategies (expressive suppression, cognitive reappraisal, and a combination) while viewing unpleasant pictures. We recorded their mood states and how successfully they used emotion regulation strategies in each block using 7-point scales. RESULTS: Relative to healthy controls, the LPP amplitudes of HUD patients were significantly lower when viewing emotional stimuli (pearlyLPP < 0.05) and using emotion regulation strategies (all p < 0.05). The left hemisphere was more active in healthy controls (pearlyLPP < 0.05, plateLPP < 0.01); there were no differences in scalp position activation among HUD patients. DISCUSSION: Compared to healthy controls, HUD patients' emotional arousal and emotion regulation ability were impaired, as reflected by the LPP component. Their abnormal scalp activation pattern may imply abnormal brain activity. Future research could explore this with electroencephalogram source analysis techniques, functional magnetic resonance imaging, or other technologies. Intervention effects for emotion dysregulation in HUD treatment are also worth exploring.


Asunto(s)
Regulación Emocional , Heroína , Electroencefalografía , Emociones , Potenciales Evocados , Humanos
10.
Curr Zool ; 67(4): 419-429, 2021 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34616939

RESUMEN

Behavioural lateralization, which reflects the functional specializations of the two brain hemispheres, is assumed to play an important role in cooperative intraspecific interactions. However, there are few studies focused on the lateralization in cooperative behaviours of individuals, especially in a natural setting. In the present study, we investigated lateralized spatial interactions between the partners in life-long monogamous pairs. The male-female pairs of two geese species (barnacle, Branta leucopsis, and white-fronted, Anser albifrons geese), were observed during different stages of the annual cycle in a variety of conditions. In geese flocks, we recorded which visual hemifield (left/right) the following partner used to monitor the leading partner relevant to the type of behaviour and the disturbance factors. In a significant majority of pairs, the following bird viewed the leading partner with the left eye during routine behaviours such as resting and feeding in undisturbed conditions. This behavioural lateralization, implicating the right hemisphere processing, was consistent across the different aggregation sites and years of the study. In contrast, no significant bias was found in a variety of geese behaviours associated with enhanced disturbance (when alert on water, flying or fleeing away when disturbed, feeding during the hunting period, in urban area feeding and during moulting). We hypothesize that the increased demands for right hemisphere processing to deal with stressful and emergency situations may interfere with the manifestation of lateralization in social interactions.

11.
R Soc Open Sci ; 8(2): 200696, 2021 Feb 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33972838

RESUMEN

A previous study we reported in this journal suggested that left and right-handers may differ in their patterns of lateralization for different language tasks (Woodhead et al. 2019 R. Soc. Open Sci. 6, 181801. (doi:10.1098/rsos.181801)). However, it had too few left-handers (N = 7) to reach firm conclusions. For this update paper, further participants were added to the sample to create separate groups of left- (N = 31) and right-handers (N = 43). Two hypotheses were tested: (1) that lateralization would be weaker at the group level in left-than right-handers; and (2) that left-handers would show weaker covariance in lateralization between tasks, supporting a two-factor model. All participants performed the same protocol as in our previous paper: lateralization was measured using functional transcranial Doppler sonography during six different language tasks, on two separate testing sessions. The results supported hypothesis 1, with significant differences in laterality between groups for four out of six tasks. For hypothesis 2, structural equation modelling showed that there was stronger evidence for a two-factor model in left than right-handers; furthermore, examination of the factor loadings suggested that the pattern of laterality across tasks may also differ between handedness groups. These results expand on what is known about the differences in laterality between left- and right-handers.

12.
Animals (Basel) ; 11(3)2021 Mar 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33799531

RESUMEN

Cerebral lateralization is a common feature present in many vertebrates and is often observed in response to various sensory stimuli. Numerous studies have proposed that some vertebrate species have a right hemisphere or left hemisphere dominance in response to specific types of acoustic stimuli. We investigated lateralization of eight giant pandas (Ailuropoda melanoleuca) by using a head turning paradigm and twenty-eight acoustic stimuli with different emotional valences which included twenty-four conspecific and four non-conspecific acoustic stimuli (white noise, thunder, and vocalization of a predator). There was no significant difference in auditory laterality in responses to conspecific or non-conspecific sounds. However, the left cerebral hemisphere processed the positive stimuli, whereas neither of the two hemispheres exhibited a preference for processing the negative stimuli. Furthermore, the right hemisphere was faster than the left hemisphere in processing emotional stimuli and conspecific stimuli. These findings demonstrate that giant pandas exhibit lateralization in response to different acoustic stimuli, which provides evidence of hemispheric asymmetry in this species.

13.
Laterality ; 26(6): 680-705, 2021 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33715589

RESUMEN

The left hemisphere is dominant for language in most people, but lateralization strength varies between different tasks and individuals. A large body of literature has shown that handedness is associated with lateralization: left handers have weaker language lateralization on average, and a greater incidence of atypical (right hemisphere) lateralization; but typically, these studies have relied on a single measure of language lateralization. Here we consider the relationships between lateralization for two different language tasks. We investigated the influence of handedness on lateralization using functional transcranial Doppler sonography (fTCD), using an existing dataset (N = 151 adults, 21 left handed). We compared a speech production task (word generation) and a semantic association task. We demonstrated stronger left-lateralization for word generation than semantic association; and a moderate correlation between laterality indices for the two tasks (r = 0.59). Laterality indices were stronger for right than left handers, and left handers were more likely than right handers to have atypical (right hemisphere) lateralization or inconsistent lateralization between the two tasks. These results add to our knowledge of individual differences in lateralization and support the view that language lateralization is multifactorial rather than unitary.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Habla , Adulto , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Semántica , Ultrasonografía Doppler Transcraneal
14.
Front Aging Neurosci ; 13: 589296, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33716706

RESUMEN

Older adults with mild or no hearing loss make more errors and expend more effort listening to speech. Cochlear implants (CI) restore hearing to deaf patients but with limited fidelity. We hypothesized that patient-reported hearing and health-related quality of life in CI patients may similarly vary according to age. Speech Spatial Qualities (SSQ) of hearing scale and Health Utilities Index Mark III (HUI) questionnaires were administered to 543 unilaterally implanted adults across Europe, South Africa, and South America. Data were acquired before surgery and at 1, 2, and 3 years post-surgery. Data were analyzed using linear mixed models with visit, age group (18-34, 35-44, 45-54, 55-64, and 65+), and side of implant as main factors and adjusted for other covariates. Tinnitus and dizziness prevalence did not vary with age, but older groups had more preoperative hearing. Preoperatively and postoperatively, SSQ scores were significantly higher (Δ0.75-0.82) for those aged <45 compared with those 55+. However, gains in SSQ scores were equivalent across age groups, although postoperative SSQ scores were higher in right-ear implanted subjects. All age groups benefited equally in terms of HUI gain (0.18), with no decrease in scores with age. Overall, younger adults appeared to cope better with a degraded hearing before and after CI, leading to better subjective hearing performance.

15.
Laterality ; 26(1-2): 1-18, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32698726

RESUMEN

In order to face a constantly changing environment, animals need to be able to update their knowledge of the world on the basis of new information. Often, this means to inhibit a previously acquired response and flexibly change their behaviour to produce a new response. Here, we measured such abilities in young domestic chicks, employing a Colour Reversal Learning Task. During the acquisition phase, 17 one-week-old male chicks had to learn to peck on one of two coloured boxes to obtain a food reward. After reaching criterion, chicks underwent a reversal phase in which the previously learned colour-reward contingency was swapped. As expected from the literature, chicks performed better in the acquisition phase with respect to the reversal phase. Results moreover highlighted the presence of a lateralized bias selectively during reversal: chicks performed better if the stimulus rewarded was located in the left hemispace (processed by the right hemisphere). Interestingly, the bias correlated with the individual difficulty, i.e., it was stronger in those chicks which needed more trials to complete the reversal session. The present study contributes evidence in support of behavioural flexibility in young chicks, along with a novel perspective on lateralized mechanisms that might underlie such ability.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Aprendizaje Inverso , Animales , Pollos , Masculino , Recompensa
16.
Handb Clin Neurol ; 173: 99-105, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32958198

RESUMEN

The present chapter offers a report on the recent literature on the neural bases of hemispheric specialization (HS), anatomical and functional developmental timecourse of HS, and on the available knowledge of their relationships with the development of handedness. Strong anatomical asymmetries can be seen located along the end of the sylvian fissure and the superior temporal sulcus (STS) as soon as the 23rd gestational week. They correspond to a leftward sulcal depth asymmetry of the Sylvian fissure coupled with a rightward asymmetry of STS. These neonatal asymmetries targeting speech processing areas do not further change with development. Different from these anatomical asymmetries, the functional asymmetries of language areas develop during childhood. Such a development is characterized at birth by a predominant interhemispheric intrinsic connectivity between homotopic areas that will evolve toward left hemisphere intrahemispheric intrinsic connectivity between anterior and posterior language poles. The development of such a typical architecture of language networks in the left hemisphere dominant for language in more than 90% of humans translates into a continuous increase in the leftward asymmetries of activation during language production throughout childhood. With regard to the rightward cerebral lateralization for visuospatial functions, neuroimaging studies tend to indicate an increase in rightward lateralization of frontal-parietal network with age during visuospatial memory and visuospatial search tasks. In addition, the spatial-attentional behavioral asymmetries emerge early (in preschool children) and, then, can be modulated by factors linked to motor asymmetry and handedness. Finally, the study of manual lateralization in relation to language development has shown the importance of considering several characteristics of manual activities. In particular, the dissociation between manipulative activities and communicative gestures in young children may open further perspectives for future research on HS.


Asunto(s)
Mapeo Encefálico , Lateralidad Funcional , Encéfalo/diagnóstico por imagen , Niño , Preescolar , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética
17.
Neuropsychologia ; 148: 107634, 2020 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32998020

RESUMEN

Pseudoneglect refers to a tendency of neurologically healthy individuals to produce leftward perceptual biases during spatial tasks, which is traditionally measured using line bisection tasks. This behavioral asymmetry is often explained as a consequence of right hemispheric dominance for visuospatial attention. The present study directly tested this notion by comparing line bisection performance between left-handers with either right hemispheric dominance (RVSD, N = 40) or "atypical" left hemispheric dominance (LVSD, N = 23) for visuospatial attention as determined by fMRI. Although we expected a reversal of pseudoneglect in participants with LVSD, our results show that they equally often err to the left of the true center compared to RVSD controls (74% of LVSD participants and 80% of RVSD participants). However, the magnitude of misbisections was found to be slightly, but significantly, smaller in the LVSD subgroup.We conclude that hemispheric asymmetry for visuospatial attention is not the main determinant of pseudoneglect as is commonly thought, but rather only constitutes one of the multiple factors which (subtly) contributes to its direction and magnitude.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Percepción Espacial , Atención , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 238(11): 2641-2651, 2020 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32924076

RESUMEN

Empirical evidence has demonstrated functional (mostly right-biased) brain asymmetry for emotion perception, whereas recent studies indicate that acute stress may modulate left and/or right hemisphere activation. However, it is still unknown whether emotion perception can be influenced by stress-induced hemispheric activation since behavioral studies report inconsistent or even contradictory results. We sought to reevaluate this gap. Eighty-eight healthy Caucasian university students participated in the study. In half of the randomly selected participants, acute psychological stress was induced by displaying a brief stressful movie clip (the stress condition), whereas the other half were shown a neutral movie clip (the non-stress condition). Prior to (the baseline) and following the movie clip display an emotion perception task was applied by presenting an emotional (happy, surprised, fearful, sad, angry, or disgusted) or neutral face to the left or right visual field. We found a more accurate perception of emotional and neutral faces presented to the LVF (the right hemisphere) in the baseline. However, we revealed that after watching a neutral movie clip, behavioral performance in emotional and neutral face perception accuracy became relatively equalized for both visual fields, whereas after watching a stressful movie clip, the RVF (the left hemisphere) even became dominant in emotional face perception. We propose a novel hemispheric functional-equivalence model: the brain is initially right-biased in emotional and neutral face perception by default; however, psychophysiological activation of a distributed brain-network due to watching neutral movie clips redistributes hemispheric performance toward relative equivalence. Moreover, even reversed hemispheric asymmetry may occur.


Asunto(s)
Reconocimiento Facial , Encéfalo , Emociones , Expresión Facial , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Estrés Psicológico
19.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 117(25): 14057-14065, 2020 06 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32513702

RESUMEN

Humans demonstrate a prototypical hemispheric functional segregation pattern, with language and praxis lateralizing to the left hemisphere and spatial attention, face recognition, and emotional prosody to the right hemisphere. In this study, we used fMRI to determine laterality for all five functions in each participant. Crucially, we recruited a sample of left-handers preselected for atypical (right) language dominance (n = 24), which allowed us to characterize hemispheric asymmetry of the other functions and compare their functional segregation pattern with that of left-handers showing typical language dominance (n = 39). Our results revealed that most participants with left language dominance display the prototypical pattern of functional hemispheric segregation (44%) or deviate from this pattern in only one function (35%). Similarly, the vast majority of right language dominant participants demonstrated a completely mirrored brain organization (50%) or a reversal for all but one cognitive function (32%). Participants deviating by more than one function from the standard segregation pattern showed poorer cognitive performance, in line with an oft-presumed biological advantage of hemispheric functional segregation.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención , Emociones , Reconocimiento Facial , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Percepción de la Altura Tonal , Percepción Espacial
20.
Neuroscience ; 425: 68-89, 2020 01 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31809726

RESUMEN

Visual self-face and proprioceptive postural recognition predominantly activate the right inferior frontoparietal cortices in human right-handers at the population level. In the present study, prompted by the finding that left-handedness may alter lateralized cortical organization for language, sensory-motor, and cognitive functions observed in right-handers, we investigated individual variations in right-dominant use of the cortices in 50 right-handers and 50 left-handers during self-body recognition (self-face and proprioceptive) tasks. We also investigated possible between-tasks differences in this right-dominant use, and possible atypical left-right reversed lateralization (right-dominance for language and left-dominance for self-body recognition) in left-handers. We measured brain activity using functional magnetic resonance imaging while participants performed a proprioceptive postural recognition task (experiencing illusory movements of the left and the right hands), a visual self-face recognition (self-other distinction) task, and a language (verb generation) task. To evaluate hemispheric dominance, we computed individual lateralization indices for the inferior frontoparietal activities in these tasks. Left-handedness altered the right-hemispheric dominance that was observed in the majority of right-handed participants in both self-body recognition tasks. In the left-handed group, during proprioceptive recognition, participants with right-lateralization, bilaterality, or left-lateralization were equally distributed, and during self-face recognition, right-lateralization was still observed, though the number of participants who demonstrated left-lateralization increased. Atypical left-right reversed lateralization was only observed in left-handed participants, but during both self-body recognition tasks. The present study provides novel and valuable knowledge about right-hemispheric dominance in self-body recognition affected by left-handedness. We discuss how functional lateralization of self-body recognition is shaped in human brain, in terms of handedness, language lateralization, and development.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Reconocimiento Facial/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Mano/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Cerebral , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Masculino
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA