Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 18 de 18
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A ; 121(25): e2405588121, 2024 Jun 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38861607

RESUMEN

Many animals can extract useful information from the vocalizations of other species. Neuroimaging studies have evidenced areas sensitive to conspecific vocalizations in the cerebral cortex of primates, but how these areas process heterospecific vocalizations remains unclear. Using fMRI-guided electrophysiology, we recorded the spiking activity of individual neurons in the anterior temporal voice patches of two macaques while they listened to complex sounds including vocalizations from several species. In addition to cells selective for conspecific macaque vocalizations, we identified an unsuspected subpopulation of neurons with strong selectivity for human voice, not merely explained by spectral or temporal structure of the sounds. The auditory representational geometry implemented by these neurons was strongly related to that measured in the human voice areas with neuroimaging and only weakly to low-level acoustical structure. These findings provide new insights into the neural mechanisms involved in auditory expertise and the evolution of communication systems in primates.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Neuronas , Vocalización Animal , Voz , Animales , Humanos , Neuronas/fisiología , Voz/fisiología , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética/métodos , Vocalización Animal/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Masculino , Macaca mulatta , Encéfalo/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Mapeo Encefálico/métodos
2.
Brain Sci ; 14(2)2024 Jan 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38391687

RESUMEN

Recent research has examined the extent to which face and voice processing are associated by virtue of the fact that both tap into a common person perception system. However, existing findings do not yet fully clarify the role of familiarity in this association. Given this, two experiments are presented that examine face-voice correlations for unfamiliar stimuli (Experiment 1) and for familiar stimuli (Experiment 2). With care being taken to use tasks that avoid floor and ceiling effects and that use realistic speech-based voice clips, the results suggested a significant positive but small-sized correlation between face and voice processing when recognizing unfamiliar individuals. In contrast, the correlation when matching familiar individuals was significant and positive, but much larger. The results supported the existing literature suggesting that face and voice processing are aligned as constituents of an overarching person perception system. However, the difference in magnitude of their association here reinforced the view that familiar and unfamiliar stimuli are processed in different ways. This likely reflects the importance of a pre-existing mental representation and cross-talk within the neural architectures when processing familiar faces and voices, and yet the reliance on more superficial stimulus-based and modality-specific analysis when processing unfamiliar faces and voices.

3.
Front Neurosci ; 17: 1217831, 2023.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37901426

RESUMEN

Background: The visual system is not fully mature at birth and continues to develop throughout infancy until it reaches adult levels through late childhood and adolescence. Disruption of vision during this postnatal period and prior to visual maturation results in deficits of visual processing and in turn may affect the development of complementary senses. Studying people who have had one eye surgically removed during early postnatal development is a useful model for understanding timelines of sensory development and the role of binocularity in visual system maturation. Adaptive auditory and audiovisual plasticity following the loss of one eye early in life has been observed for both low-and high-level visual stimuli. Notably, people who have had one eye removed early in life perceive the McGurk effect much less than binocular controls. Methods: The current study investigates whether multisensory compensatory mechanisms are also present in people who had one eye removed late in life, after postnatal visual system maturation, by measuring whether they perceive the McGurk effect compared to binocular controls and people who have had one eye removed early in life. Results: People who had one eye removed late in life perceived the McGurk effect similar to binocular viewing controls, unlike those who had one eye removed early in life. Conclusion: This suggests differences in multisensory compensatory mechanisms based on age at surgical eye removal. These results indicate that cross-modal adaptations for the loss of binocularity may be dependent on plasticity levels during cortical development.

4.
Neuropsychologia ; 173: 108312, 2022 08 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35781011

RESUMEN

The recognition of human speakers by their voices is a remarkable cognitive ability. Previous research has established a voice area in the right temporal cortex involved in the integration of speaker-specific acoustic features. This integration appears to occur rapidly, especially in case of familiar voices. However, the exact time course of this process is less well understood. To this end, we here investigated the automatic change detection response of the human brain while listening to the famous voice of German chancellor Angela Merkel, embedded in the context of acoustically matched voices. A classic passive oddball paradigm contrasted short word stimuli uttered by Merkel with word stimuli uttered by two unfamiliar female speakers. Electrophysiological voice processing indices from 21 participants were quantified as mismatch negativities (MMNs) and P3a differences. Cortical sources were approximated by variable resolution electromagnetic tomography. The results showed amplitude and latency effects for both MMN and P3a: The famous (familiar) voice elicited a smaller but earlier MMN than the unfamiliar voices. The P3a, by contrast, was both larger and later for the familiar than for the unfamiliar voices. Familiar-voice MMNs originated from right-hemispheric regions in temporal cortex, overlapping with the temporal voice area, while unfamiliar-voice MMNs stemmed from left superior temporal gyrus. These results suggest that the processing of a very famous voice relies on pre-attentive right temporal processing within the first 150 ms of the acoustic signal. The findings further our understanding of the neural dynamics underlying familiar voice processing.


Asunto(s)
Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Atención , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Voz/fisiología
5.
Neuropsychologia ; 158: 107902, 2021 07 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34052231

RESUMEN

This study investigated the impact of the speaker's identity generated by the voice on sentence processing. We examined the relation between ERP components associated with the processing of the voice (N100 and P200) from voice onset and those associated with sentence processing (N400 and late positivity) from critical word onset. We presented Dutch native speakers with sentences containing true (and known) information, unknown (but true) information or information violating world knowledge and had them perform a truth evaluation task. Sentences were spoken either in a native or a foreign accent. Truth evaluation judgments were not different for statements spoken by the native-accented and the foreign-accented speakers. Reduced N100 and P200 were observed in response to the foreign speaker's voice compared to the native speaker's. While statements containing unknown information or world knowledge violations generated a larger N400 than true statements in the native condition, they were not significantly different in the foreign condition, suggesting shallower processing of foreign-accented speech. The N100 was a significant predictor for the N400 in that the reduced N100 observed for the foreign speaker compared to the native speaker was related to a smaller N400 effect. These finding suggest that the impression of the speaker that listeners rapidly form from the voice affects semantic processing, which confirms that speaker's identity and language comprehension cannot be dissociated.


Asunto(s)
Electroencefalografía , Percepción del Habla , Potenciales Evocados , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Habla
6.
Orphanet J Rare Dis ; 15(1): 22, 2020 01 20.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31959191

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Prader-Willi syndrome (PWS) is a rare and complex neurodevelopmental disorder of genetic origin. It manifests itself in endocrine and cognitive problems, including highly pronounced hyperphagia and severe obesity. In many cases, impaired acquisition of social and communication skills leads to autism spectrum features, and individuals with this syndrome are occasionally diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) using specific scales. Given that communicational skills are largely based on vocal communication, it is important to study human voice processing in PWS. We were able to examine a large number of participants with PWS (N = 61) recruited from France's national reference center for PWS and other hospitals. We tested their voice and nonvoice recognition abilities, as well as their ability to distinguish between voices and nonvoices in a free choice task. We applied the hierarchical drift diffusion model (HDDM) with Bayesian estimation to compare decision-making in participants with PWS and controls. RESULTS: We found that PWS participants were impaired on both voice and nonvoice processing, but displayed a compensatory ability to perceive voices. Participants with uniparental disomy had poorer voice and nonvoice perception than participants with a deletion on chromosome 15. The HDDM allowed us to demonstrate that participants with PWS need to accumulate more information in order to make a decision, are slower at decision-making, and are predisposed to voice perception, albeit to a lesser extent than controls. CONCLUSIONS: The categorization of voices and nonvoices is generally preserved in participants with PWS, though this may not be the case for the lowest IQ.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno del Espectro Autista , Síndrome de Prader-Willi , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/genética , Teorema de Bayes , Humanos , Síndrome de Prader-Willi/genética , Disomía Uniparental
7.
Sensors (Basel) ; 19(20)2019 Oct 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31658784

RESUMEN

In Brazil, the recognition of speakers for forensic purposes still relies on a subjectivity-based decision-making process through a results analysis of untrustworthy techniques. Owing to the lack of a voice database, speaker verification is currently applied to samples specifically collected for confrontation. However, speaker comparative analysis via contested discourse requires the collection of an excessive amount of voice samples for a series of individuals. Further, the recognition system must inform who is the most compatible with the contested voice from pre-selected individuals. Accordingly, this paper proposes using a combination of linear predictive coding (LPC) and ordinary least squares (OLS) as a speaker verification tool for forensic analysis. The proposed recognition technique establishes confidence and similarity upon which to base forensic reports, indicating verification of the speaker of the contested discourse. Therefore, in this paper, an accurate, quick, alternative method to help verify the speaker is contributed. After running seven different tests, this study preliminarily achieved a hit rate of 100% considering a limited dataset (Brazilian Portuguese). Furthermore, the developed method extracts a larger number of formants, which are indispensable for statistical comparisons via OLS. The proposed framework is robust at certain levels of noise, for sentences with the suppression of word changes, and with different quality or even meaningful audio time differences.

8.
Behav Brain Res ; 356: 89-97, 2019 01 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30081041

RESUMEN

Several studies report sex differences in sensitivity to gendered stimuli. We assume many of these to reflect differences as to the sex to which one feels attracted rather than to biological sex per se. Investigating voice perception, a function of high social relevance, we show that the behavioural and neural (BOLD) responses to male and female voices are mediated by sex and sexual orientation. In heterosexual men and women, we found an opposite-sex effect, reflected in higher classification accuracy for and a response bias towards voices of the other sex, while the effect became apparent as same-sex effect in homosexual men and women. Overall, sexual orientation had a greater impact in women than in men and homosexual women were closer to men in their behavioural responses to female voices. The activation patterns were similar for hetero- and homosexual men, both groups showing increased activation in response to male compared to female voices in regions distributed across the temporo-parietal and insular cortex. In contrast, women had increased activation in response to voices of the desired sex. It appears that both sex and sexual orientation impact on a function as basal as voice perception. Our results underline the need to assess sexual orientation in study participants if conclusions on sex differences shall be drawn. Many of the reported sex differences in behaviour and brain function might be mediated by sexual orientation and we encourage further research into the interplay between sex and sexual orientation.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Emociones/fisiología , Conducta Sexual/fisiología , Adulto , Encéfalo/fisiología , Mapeo Encefálico , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Femenino , Heterosexualidad , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Caracteres Sexuales , Factores Sexuales , Minorías Sexuales y de Género , Habla , Voz/fisiología , Adulto Joven
9.
Neurosci Lett ; 672: 103-107, 2018 04 13.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29474874

RESUMEN

Previously, we have shown that people who have had one eye surgically removed early in life during visual development have enhanced sound localization [1] and lack visual dominance, commonly observed in binocular and monocular (eye-patched) viewing controls [2]. Despite these changes, people with one eye integrate auditory and visual components of multisensory events optimally [3]. The current study investigates how people with one eye perceive the McGurk effect, an audiovisual illusion where a new syllable is perceived when visual lip movements do not match the corresponding sound [4]. We compared individuals with one eye to binocular and monocular viewing controls and found that they have a significantly smaller McGurk effect compared to binocular controls. Additionally, monocular controls tended to perceive the McGurk effect less often than binocular controls suggesting a small transient modulation of the McGurk effect. These results suggest altered weighting of the auditory and visual modalities with both short and long-term monocular viewing. These results indicate the presence of permanent adaptive perceptual accommodations in people who have lost one eye early in life that may serve to mitigate the loss of binocularity during early brain development.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Ilusiones/fisiología , Visión Monocular/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Localización de Sonidos/fisiología , Adulto Joven
10.
Neuropsychologia ; 116(Pt B): 194-204, 2018 07 31.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28733246

RESUMEN

Twenty-nine patients who underwent surgery for a temporal glioma, either in the left (16 patients) or right (13 patients) hemisphere, were administered standardized tests of unknown voice discrimination (UVD) and of famous voice recognition (VO-REC), which included tasks of familiarity evaluation, semantic identification and naming of famous voices. The UVD consisted of twenty stimuli, in which two audio files were consecutively presented; the subject was requested to judge whether the voices belonged to the same or different persons. In the VO-REC, patients were requested to recognize the voices of 40 very well known people; these voices were intermingled with the voices of 20 unknown people for a familiarity check, followed by identification and naming of persons recognized as familiar. We aimed at verifying the effect of laterality and intra-temporal site of lesion on familiarity assessment, false alarms, identification and naming of familiar people. As for the effect of lesion side, our results showed that patients with right temporal gliomas were significantly more impaired in voices discrimination and produced more false alarms than patients with a left glioma, who, in turn, were significantly more impaired in name retrieval than patients with a right temporal glioma. The high number of false alarms in patients with a right temporal glioma suggests that familiarity judgment was impaired. Regarding the neuroanatomical correlates of these different patterns of impairment, MRI data suggested that: (a) UVD disorders are due to lesions involving the whole right anterior temporal lobe and extending to lateral portions of the temporal and frontal lobes; (b) familiarity judgments (testified by an increased number of false alarms) are impaired in lesions restricted to the right anterior temporal lobe; (c) name retrieval deficits are found only in patients with left temporal lesions. UVD disorders were interpreted, at least in part, as due to an impairment of executive functions, resulting from a disconnection of the right temporal lobe from the frontal lobe control. A partly unexpected finding was that some patients with a right temporal tumour had a normal performance in famous voice recognition and identification, in spite of having severe voice discrimination disturbances. These unexpected results, in agreement with previous observation made in the visual (face) modality, are inconsistent with strictly hierarchical models of voice processing.


Asunto(s)
Neoplasias Encefálicas/complicaciones , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Glioma/complicaciones , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/patología , Adulto , Anciano , Neoplasias Encefálicas/diagnóstico por imagen , Neoplasias Encefálicas/patología , Discriminación en Psicología , Personajes , Femenino , Glioma/diagnóstico por imagen , Glioma/patología , Humanos , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Nombres , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estudios Retrospectivos , Lóbulo Temporal/diagnóstico por imagen , Voz , Adulto Joven
11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26442245

RESUMEN

Person identification, especially in critical environments, has always been a subject of great interest. However, it has gained a new dimension in a world threatened by a new kind of terrorism that uses social networks (e.g., YouTube) to broadcast its message. In this new scenario, classical identification methods (such as fingerprints or face recognition) have been forcedly replaced by alternative biometric characteristics such as voice, as sometimes this is the only feature available. The present study benefits from the advances achieved during last years in understanding and modeling voice production. The paper hypothesizes that a gender-dependent characterization of speakers combined with the use of a set of features derived from the components, resulting from the deconstruction of the voice into its glottal source and vocal tract estimates, will enhance recognition rates when compared to classical approaches. A general description about the main hypothesis and the methodology followed to extract the gender-dependent extended biometric parameters is given. Experimental validation is carried out both on a highly controlled acoustic condition database, and on a mobile phone network recorded under non-controlled acoustic conditions.

12.
Cortex ; 71: 122-33, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26200892

RESUMEN

Adults diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) show a reduced sensitivity (degree of selective response) to social stimuli such as human voices. In order to determine whether this reduced sensitivity is a consequence of years of poor social interaction and communication or is present prior to significant experience, we used functional MRI to examine cortical sensitivity to auditory stimuli in infants at high familial risk for later emerging ASD (HR group, N = 15), and compared this to infants with no family history of ASD (LR group, N = 18). The infants (aged between 4 and 7 months) were presented with voice and environmental sounds while asleep in the scanner and their behaviour was also examined in the context of observed parent-infant interaction. Whereas LR infants showed early specialisation for human voice processing in right temporal and medial frontal regions, the HR infants did not. Similarly, LR infants showed stronger sensitivity than HR infants to sad vocalisations in the right fusiform gyrus and left hippocampus. Also, in the HR group only, there was an association between each infant's degree of engagement during social interaction and the degree of voice sensitivity in key cortical regions. These results suggest that at least some infants at high-risk for ASD have atypical neural responses to human voice with and without emotional valence. Further exploration of the relationship between behaviour during social interaction and voice processing may help better understand the mechanisms that lead to different outcomes in at risk populations.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Trastorno del Espectro Autista/psicología , Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Emociones , Femenino , Hipocampo/fisiopatología , Humanos , Lactante , Relaciones Interpersonales , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Relaciones Madre-Hijo , Riesgo , Sueño , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología
13.
Neurosci Lett ; 593: 35-9, 2015 Apr 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25766754

RESUMEN

Several studies have identified, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), a region within the superior temporal gyrus that preferentially responds to musical stimuli. However, in most cases, significant responses to other complex stimuli, particularly human voice, were also observed. Thus, it remains unknown if the same neurons respond to both stimulus types, albeit with different strengths, or whether the responses observed with fMRI are generated by distinct, overlapping neural populations. To address this question, we conducted an fMRI experiment in which short music excerpts and human vocalizations were presented in a pseudo-random order. Critically, we performed an adaptation-based analysis in which responses to the stimuli were analyzed taking into account the category of the preceding stimulus. Our results confirm the presence of a region in the anterior STG that responds more strongly to music than voice. Moreover, we found a music-specific adaptation effect in this area, consistent with the existence of music-preferred neurons. Lack of differences between musicians and non-musicians argues against an expertise effect. These findings provide further support for neural separability between music and speech within the temporal lobe.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva , Encéfalo/fisiología , Música , Voz/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adaptación Psicológica , Adulto , Mapeo Encefálico , Cara , Femenino , Humanos , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Percepción Visual
14.
Front Hum Neurosci ; 8: 499, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25071527

RESUMEN

Regions along the superior temporal sulci and in the anterior temporal lobes have been found to be involved in voice processing. It has even been argued that parts of the temporal cortices serve as voice-selective areas. Yet, evidence for voice-selective activation in the strict sense is still missing. The current fMRI study aimed at assessing the degree of voice-specific processing in different parts of the superior and middle temporal cortices. To this end, voices of famous persons were contrasted with widely different categories, which were sounds of animals and musical instruments. The argumentation was that only brain regions with statistically proven absence of activation by the control stimuli may be considered as candidates for voice-selective areas. Neural activity was found to be stronger in response to human voices in all analyzed parts of the temporal lobes except for the middle and posterior STG. More importantly, the activation differences between voices and the other environmental sounds increased continuously from the mid-posterior STG to the anterior MTG. Here, only voices but not the control stimuli excited an increase of the BOLD response above a resting baseline level. The findings are discussed with reference to the function of the anterior temporal lobes in person recognition and the general question on how to define selectivity of brain regions for a specific class of stimuli or tasks. In addition, our results corroborate recent assumptions about the hierarchical organization of auditory processing building on a processing stream from the primary auditory cortices to anterior portions of the temporal lobes.

15.
J Speech Lang Hear Res ; 56(5): 1389-401, 2013 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24023381

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: In this study, the authors examined the ability of subjects with cochlear implants (CIs) to discriminate voice gender and how this ability evolved as a function of CI experience. METHOD: The authors presented a continuum of voice samples created by voice morphing, with 9 intermediate acoustic parameter steps between a typical male and a typical female. This method allowed for the evaluation of gender categorization not only when acoustical features were specific to gender but also for more ambiguous cases, when fundamental frequency or formant distribution were located between typical values. RESULTS: Results showed a global, though variable, deficit for voice gender categorization in CI recipients compared with subjects with normal hearing. This deficit was stronger for ambiguous stimuli in the voice continuum: Average performance scores for CI users were 58% lower than average scores for subjects with normal hearing in cases of ambiguous stimuli and 19% lower for typical male and female voices. The authors found no significant improvement in voice gender categorization with CI experience. CONCLUSIONS: These results emphasize the dissociation between recovery of speech recognition and voice feature perception after cochlear implantation. This large and durable deficit may be related to spectral and temporal degradation induced by CI sound coding, or it may be related to central voice processing deficits.


Asunto(s)
Implantación Coclear/rehabilitación , Implantes Cocleares , Sordera/rehabilitación , Caracteres Sexuales , Percepción del Habla , Voz , Estimulación Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Femenino , Estudios de Seguimiento , Audición , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Psicoacústica , Psicometría , Acústica del Lenguaje , Adulto Joven
16.
Psychol Sci ; 24(11): 2297-305, 2013 Nov 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24068117

RESUMEN

Exposure, or adaptation, to faces or voices biases perceptions of subsequent stimuli, for example, causing faces to appear more normal than they would be otherwise if they are similar to the previously presented stimuli. Studies also suggest that there may be cross-modal adaptation between sound and vision, although the evidence is inconsistent. We examined adaptation effects within and across voices and faces and also tested whether adaptation crosses between male and female stimuli. We exposed participants to sex-typical or sex-atypical stimuli and measured the perceived normality of subsequent stimuli. Exposure to female faces or voices altered perceptions of subsequent female stimuli, and these adaptation effects crossed modality; exposure to voices influenced judgments of faces, and vice versa. We also found that exposure to female stimuli did not influence perception of subsequent male stimuli. Our data demonstrate that recent experience of faces and voices changes subsequent perception and that mental representations of faces and voices may not be modality dependent. Both unimodal and cross-modal adaptation effects appear to be relatively sex-specific.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Psicológica/fisiología , Cara , Percepción/fisiología , Voz , Adulto , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores Sexuales , Percepción Social , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
17.
Brain Res ; 1528: 20-7, 2013 Aug 28.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23820425

RESUMEN

Electrophysiological correlates of voice processing were studied in twenty adults by comparing auditory evoked potentials in response to voice and environmental sounds in passive condition. Both categories of stimuli elicited similar cortical auditory responses (i.e. N1, P2, N2 peaks); however these peaks were overlapped by two components specifically elicited by voice. The first component was evidenced as a positive deflection recorded over the fronto-temporal sites, and lateralized on the right hemiscalp. This fronto-temporal positivity to voice (FTPV) may constitute the electrophysiological counterpart of the activation of the temporal voice areas previously described in neuroimaging studies. The second component was recorded at occipito-temporo-parietal sites. This occipito-temporo-parietal negativity to voice might correspond to visual mental imagery of the vocal sounds or to some form of mental simulation of the action sounds (e.g. coughing). Both components began as early as 70 ms post-stimulus onset indicating a rapid discrimination of voice in our auditory environment, which might be the basis of communication functions in humans.


Asunto(s)
Potenciales Evocados Auditivos , Lóbulo Frontal/fisiología , Lóbulo Occipital/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
18.
Soc Neurosci ; 7(3): 317-30, 2012.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21950945

RESUMEN

How specialized is the infant brain for processing voice within our environment? Research in adults suggests that portions of the temporal lobe play an important role in differentiating vocalizations from other environmental sounds; however, very little is known about this process in infancy. Recent research in infants has revealed discrepancies in the cortical location of voice-selective activation, as well as the age of onset of this response. The current study used functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) to further investigate voice processing in awake 4-7-month-old infants. In listening to voice and non-voice sounds, there was robust and widespread activation in bilateral temporal cortex. Further, voice-selective regions of the bilateral anterior temporal cortex evidenced a steady increase in voice selective activation (voice > non-voice activation) over 4-7 months of age. These findings support a growing body of evidence that the emergence of cerebral specialization for human voice sounds evolves over the first 6 months of age.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Encéfalo/crecimiento & desarrollo , Encéfalo/metabolismo , Voz , Estimulación Acústica , Mapeo Encefálico , Femenino , Humanos , Lactante , Masculino , Oxihemoglobinas , Espectroscopía Infrarroja Corta , Factores de Tiempo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA