Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 34
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Med Phys ; 2024 Jul 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-39008812

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Lesion detection is one of the most important clinical tasks in positron emission tomography (PET) for oncology. An anthropomorphic model observer (MO) designed to replicate human observers (HOs) in a detection task is an important tool for assessing task-based image quality. The channelized Hotelling observer (CHO) has been the most popular anthropomorphic MO. Recently, deep learning MOs (DLMOs), mostly based on convolutional neural networks (CNNs), have been investigated for various imaging modalities. However, there have been few studies on DLMOs for PET. PURPOSE: The goal of the study is to investigate whether DLMOs can predict HOs better than conventional MOs such as CHO in a two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) detection task using PET images with real anatomical variability. METHODS: Two types of DLMOs were implemented: (1) CNN DLMO, and (2) CNN-SwinT DLMO that combines CNN and Swin Transformer (SwinT) encoders. Lesion-absent PET images were reconstructed from clinical data, and lesion-present images were reconstructed with adding simulated lesion sinogram data. Lesion-present and lesion-absent PET image pairs were labeled by eight HOs consisting of four radiologists and four image scientists in a 2AFC detection task. In total, 2268 pairs of lesion-present and lesion-absent images were used for training, 324 pairs for validation, and 324 pairs for test. CNN DLMO, CNN-SwinT DLMO, CHO with internal noise, and non-prewhitening matched filter (NPWMF) were compared in the same train-test paradigm. For comparison, six quantitative metrics including prediction accuracy, mean squared errors (MSEs) and correlation coefficients, which measure how well a MO predicts HOs, were calculated in a 9-fold cross-validation experiment. RESULTS: In terms of the accuracy and MSE metrics, CNN DLMO and CNN-SwinT DLMO showed better performance than CHO and NPWMF, and CNN-SwinT DLMO showed the best performance among the MOs evaluated. CONCLUSIONS: DLMO can predict HOs more accurately than conventional MOs such as CHO in PET lesion detection. Combining SwinT and CNN encoders can improve the DLMO prediction performance compared to using CNN only.

2.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38799476

RESUMEN

Undersampling in the frequency domain (k-space) in MRI enables faster data acquisition. In this study, we used a fixed 1D undersampling factor of 5x with only 20% of the k-space collected. The fraction of fully acquired low k-space frequencies were varied from 0% (all aliasing) to 20% (all blurring). The images were reconstructed using a multi-coil SENSE algorithm. We used two-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) and the forced localization tasks with a subtle signal to estimate the human observer performance. The 2-AFC average human observer performance remained fairly constant across all imaging conditions. The forced localization task performance improved from the 0% condition to the 2.5% condition and remained fairly constant for the remaining conditions, suggesting that there was a decrease in task performance only in the pure aliasing situation. We modeled the average human performance using a sparse-difference of Gaussians (SDOG) Hotelling observer model. Because the blurring in the undersampling direction makes the mean signal asymmetric, we explored an adaptation for irregular signals that made the SDOG template asymmetric. To improve the observer performance, we also varied the number of SDOG channels from 3 to 4. We found that despite the asymmetry in the mean signal, both the symmetric and asymmetric models reasonably predicted the human performance in the 2-AFC experiments. However, the symmetric model performed slightly better. We also found that a symmetric SDOG model with 4 channels implemented using a spatial domain convolution and constrained to the possible signal locations reasonably modeled the forced localization human observer results.

3.
Elife ; 132024 Jan 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38224341

RESUMEN

An abundant literature reports on 'sequential effects' observed when humans make predictions on the basis of stochastic sequences of stimuli. Such sequential effects represent departures from an optimal, Bayesian process. A prominent explanation posits that humans are adapted to changing environments, and erroneously assume non-stationarity of the environment, even if the latter is static. As a result, their predictions fluctuate over time. We propose a different explanation in which sub-optimal and fluctuating predictions result from cognitive constraints (or costs), under which humans however behave rationally. We devise a framework of costly inference, in which we develop two classes of models that differ by the nature of the constraints at play: in one case the precision of beliefs comes at a cost, resulting in an exponential forgetting of past observations, while in the other beliefs with high predictive power are favored. To compare model predictions to human behavior, we carry out a prediction task that uses binary random stimuli, with probabilities ranging from 0.05 to 0.95. Although in this task the environment is static and the Bayesian belief converges, subjects' predictions fluctuate and are biased toward the recent stimulus history. Both classes of models capture this 'attractive effect', but they depart in their characterization of higher-order effects. Only the precision-cost model reproduces a 'repulsive effect', observed in the data, in which predictions are biased away from stimuli presented in more distant trials. Our experimental results reveal systematic modulations in sequential effects, which our theoretical approach accounts for in terms of rationality under cognitive constraints.


Asunto(s)
Teorema de Bayes , Humanos , Probabilidad
4.
Mem Cognit ; 51(8): 1836-1848, 2023 11.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37326785

RESUMEN

The changing state effect is the finding that a stream of irrelevant sounds that change more (e.g., different digits in random order) disrupts memory more than a stream of irrelevant sounds that change less (e.g., a single digit repeated over and over). According to the Object-Oriented Episodic Record (O-OER) model, the changing state effect will be observed only in memory tasks that have an order component or which induce serial rehearsal or serial processing. In contrast, other accounts-including the Feature Model, the Primacy Model, and various attentional theories-predict that the changing state effect should be observable when there is no order component. Experiment 1 first demonstrated that the irrelevant stimuli created for the current experiments produced a changing state effect in immediate serial recall in both on-campus and online samples. Then, three experiments assessed whether a changing state effect is observable in a surprise 2AFC recognition test. Experiment 2 replicated Stokes and Arnell (2012, Memory & Cognition, 40, 918-931), who found that although irrelevant sounds reduce performance on a surprise recognition test of words presented previously in a lexical decision task, they do not produce a changing state effect. Experiments 3 and 4 used two different encoding tasks (pleasantness and frequency judgment) and also found no changing state effect. The results support the prediction of the O-OER model and provide additional evidence against the other accounts.


Asunto(s)
Percepción del Habla , Habla , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental , Aprendizaje , Memoria a Corto Plazo
5.
Nutrients ; 15(10)2023 May 22.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37242298

RESUMEN

Individual taste sensitivity influences food preferences, nutritional control, and health, and differs greatly between individuals. The purpose of this study was to establish a method of measuring and quantifying an individual's taste sensitivity and to evaluate the relationship between taste variation and genetic polymorphisms in humans using agonist specificities of the bitter taste receptor gene, TAS2R38, with the bitter compound 6-n-propylthiouracil (PROP). We precisely detected the threshold of PROP bitter perception by conducting the modified two-alternative forced-choice (2AFC) procedure with the Bayesian staircase procedure of the QUEST method and examined genetic variation in TAS2R38 in a Japanese population. There were significant differences in PROP threshold between the three TAS2R38 genotype pairs for 79 subjects: PAV/PAV vs AVI/AVI, p < 0.001; PAV/AVI vs AVI/AVI, p < 0.001; and PAV/PAV vs PAV/AVI, p < 0.01. Our results quantified individual bitter perception as QUEST threshold values: the PROP bitter perception of individuals with the PAV/PAV or PAV/AVI genotypes was tens to fifty times more sensitive than that of an individual with the AVI/AVI genotype. Our analyses provide a basic model for the accurate estimation of taste thresholds using the modified 2AFC with the QUEST approach.


Asunto(s)
Umbral Gustativo , Gusto , Adulto , Humanos , Gusto/genética , Umbral Gustativo/genética , Propiltiouracilo , Japón , Teorema de Bayes , Receptores Acoplados a Proteínas G/genética , Percepción del Gusto/genética , Genotipo , Polimorfismo Genético , Variación Genética
6.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37131343

RESUMEN

Undersampling in the frequency domain (k-space) in MRI accelerates the data acquisition. Typically, a fraction of the low frequencies is fully collected and the rest are equally undersampled. We used a fixed 1D undersampling factor of 5x where 20% of the k-space lines are collected but varied the fraction of the low k-space frequencies that are fully sampled. We used a range of fully acquired low k-space frequencies from 0% where the primary artifact is aliasing to 20% where the primary artifact is blurring in the undersampling direction. Small lesions were placed in the coil k-space data for fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) brain images from the fastMRI database. The images were reconstructed using a multi-coil SENSE reconstruction with no regularization. We conducted a human observer two-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) study with a signal known exactly and a search task with variable backgrounds for each of the acquisitions. We found that for the 2-AFC task, the average human observer did better with more of the low frequencies being fully sampled. For the search task, we found that after an initial improvement from having none of the low frequencies fully sampled to just 2.5%, the performance remained fairly constant. We found that the performance in the two tasks had a different relationship to the acquired data. We also found that the search task was more consistent with common practice in MRI where a range of frequencies between 5% and 10% of the low frequencies are fully sampled.

7.
Cogn Sci ; 47(4): e13268, 2023 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37062829

RESUMEN

Iconicity, or the resemblance between form and meaning, is often ascribed to a special status and contrasted with default assumptions of arbitrariness in spoken language. But does iconicity in spoken language have a special status when it comes to learnability? A simple way to gauge learnability is to see how well something is retrieved from memory. We can further contrast this with guessability, to see (1) whether the ease of guessing the meanings of ideophones outperforms the rate at which they are remembered; and (2) how willing participants' are to reassess what they were taught in a prior task-a novel contribution of this study. We replicate prior guessing and memory tasks using ideophones and adjectives from Japanese, Korean, and Igbo. Our results show that although native Cantonese speakers guessed ideophone meanings above chance level, they memorized both ideophones and adjectives with comparable accuracy. However, response time data show that participants took significantly longer to respond correctly to adjective-meaning pairs-indicating a discrepancy in a cognitive effort that favored the recognition of ideophones. In a follow-up reassessment task, participants who were taught foil translations were more likely to choose the true translations for ideophones rather than adjectives. By comparing the findings from our guessing and memory tasks, we conclude that iconicity is more accessible if a task requires participants to actively seek out sound-meaning associations.


Asunto(s)
Lenguaje , Sonido , Humanos , Recuerdo Mental
8.
Food Chem ; 368: 130849, 2022 Jan 30.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34419796

RESUMEN

Umami intensity promotes food flavor blending and food choice, while a universal quantification procedure is still lacking. To evaluate perceived umami intensity (PUI) in seven categories of foods, modified two-alternative forced choice (2-AFC) method with monosodium glutamate as reference was applied. Meanwhile, we explored whether equivalent umami concentration (EUC) by chemical analysis and electronic tongue (E-tongue) are applicable in PUI quantification. The results indicated that EUC was appropriate in quantifying PUI of samples from meat, dairy, vegetable and mushroom groups (r = 1.00, p < 0.05). Moreover, models with a good prediction capacity for PUI and EUC (R2 > 0.99) were established in separated food categories by back propagation neural networks, where E-tongue data were set as input. This study explored the effectiveness of the three methods in evaluating the PUIs of various foods, which provides multiple choices for the food industry.


Asunto(s)
Nariz Electrónica , Gusto , Aromatizantes , Aditivos Alimentarios , Glutamato de Sodio
9.
Prog Brain Res ; 262: 115-137, 2021.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33931175

RESUMEN

Tinnitus assessment is a precursor for individualized treatment and outcome measurement. In the recent years, several studies proposed two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) recursive matching as a method to determine tinnitus pitch-match frequency in a standardized reliable manner. Currently, pure tones are used as comparison stimuli to assess pitch-match frequency. In this study, we investigated the psychometric quality of the method comparing different sound types. We measured 20 chronic tinnitus patients in 2 runs on 3 days. To assess pitch-match frequency, we used 2AFC recursive matching and compared results between pure tones and narrow band noise (NBN). Test-retest reliability between runs and across sound types was high (α>0.9) and increased across days. Perceived matching difficulty and time to completion decreased over repetitions. Importantly, the difference of matched frequencies (DMF) between runs was significantly less for NBN. When patients matched the spectral bandwidth of a test tone to their tinnitus, consistency was high (α=0.86) and no patient indicated continuously a pure tone. In conclusion, we recommend using NBN sounds in 2AFC recursive matching to assess pitch-match frequency as a standardized reliable method. Such a procedure could be offered as smartphone-based application to monitor tinnitus symptomatology for individualized assessment and treatment outcome.


Asunto(s)
Acúfeno , Audiometría , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Resultado del Tratamiento
10.
Front Behav Neurosci ; 14: 159, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33088265

RESUMEN

Pupillometry, the measure of pupil size and reactivity, has been widely used to assess cognitive processes. Changes in pupil size have been shown to correlate with various behavioral states, both externally and internally induced such as locomotion, arousal, cortical state, and decision-making processes. Besides, these pupillary responses have also been linked to the activity of neuromodulatory systems that modulate attention and perception such as the noradrenergic and cholinergic systems. Due to the extent of processes the pupil reflects, we aimed at further resolving pupillary responses in the context of behavioral state and task performance while recording pupillary transients of mice performing a vibrotactile two-alternative forced-choice task (2-AFC). We show that before the presentation of task-relevant information, pre-stimulus, pupil size differentiates between states of disengagement from task performance vs. engagement. Also, when subjects have to attend to task stimuli to attain a reward, post-stimulus, pupillary dilations exhibit a difference between correct and error responses with this difference reflecting an internal decision variable. We hypothesize that this internal decision variable relates to response confidence, the internal perception of the confidence the subject has in its choice. As opposed to this, we show that in a condition of passive performance, when the stimulus has no more task relevance due to reward being provided automatically, pupillary dilations reflect the occurrence of stimulation and reward provision but not decisional variables as under active performance. Our results provide evidence that in addition to reflecting attentiveness under task performance rather than arousal per se, pupil dilations also reflect the confidence of the subject in his ensuing response. This confidence coding is overlaid within a more pronounced pupil dilation that reflects post-decision components that are related to the response itself but not to the decision. We also provide evidence as to how different behavioral states, imposed by task demands, modulate what the pupil is reflecting, presumably showing what the underlying cognitive network is coding for.

11.
J Exp Biol ; 223(Pt 18)2020 09 23.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32816960

RESUMEN

Every day nectar-feeding animals face an energetic challenge during foraging: they must locate and select flowers that provide nectar with adequate amounts of sugar to cover their very high energy needs. To understand this decision-making process, it is crucial to know how accurately sugar concentration differences can be discriminated. In a controlled laboratory setting, we offered the nectar-specialist bat Leptonycteris yerbabuenae the choice between different sugar solutions covering the entire concentration range of bat-pollinated plants (3-33%). When feeding on solutions below 10% sugar concentration, L. yerbabuenae were unable to cover their energetic demands because of physiological constraints. Their ability to discriminate sugar concentrations was better than that of any other nectar-feeding animal studied to date. At sugar concentrations below 15%, L. yerbabuenae can discriminate solutions differing by only 0.5%. The bats may utilize this fine-tuned ability to select nectar from flowers with reward qualities that provide them with the necessary amount of energy to survive.


Asunto(s)
Quirópteros , Néctar de las Plantas , Animales , Carbohidratos , Conducta Alimentaria , Azúcares
12.
Front Mol Neurosci ; 13: 30, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32180705

RESUMEN

The cortex is crucial for many behaviors, ranging from sensory-based behaviors to working memory and social behaviors. To gain an in-depth understanding of the contribution to these behaviors, cellular and sub-cellular recordings from both individual and populations of cortical neurons are vital. However, techniques allowing such recordings, such as two-photon imaging and whole-cell electrophysiology, require absolute stability of the head, a requirement not often fulfilled in freely moving animals. Here, we review and compare behavioral paradigms that have been developed and adapted for the head-fixed preparation, which together offer the needed stability for live recordings of neural activity in behaving animals. We also review how the head-fixed preparation has been used to explore the function of primary sensory cortices, posterior parietal cortex (PPC) and anterior lateral motor (ALM) cortex in sensory-based behavioral tasks, while also discussing the considerations of performing such recordings. Overall, this review highlights the head-fixed preparation as allowing in-depth investigation into the neural activity underlying behaviors by providing highly controllable settings for precise stimuli presentation which can be combined with behavioral paradigms ranging from simple sensory detection tasks to complex, cross-modal, memory-guided decision-making tasks.

13.
Cognition ; 197: 104201, 2020 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31999956

RESUMEN

Although it remains contentious, women's changeable attraction to masculine faces has been used to inform evolutionary ideas about human mating strategies. Typical experiments in this area use two-alternative-forced-choice (2afc) over a few pairs of similar images. The reliability of these measures is analysed suggesting that many studies have too few trials to be reliable. In the current experiment, fertility shifts in preferences for masculinised faces (and Africanised faces) were explored using both attractiveness ratings and a 2afc method over 80 pairs. The 2afc method showed a fertility shift in preferences whereas attractiveness ratings did not show a shift. Further, it was demonstrated how the size of the preferences shown in the 2afc tasks correlated with general face-matching performance. It is concluded that fertility is associated with improved face-processing accuracy and hence 2afc designs have poor validity as measures of masculinity preference. These issues of validity and reliability may have contributed to the contentious nature of fertility effects on preferences. Further, validity and reliability need to be considered in any study where a change in preference is identified using a comparative-preference task.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Masculinidad , Femenino , Fertilidad , Humanos , Masculino , Ciclo Menstrual , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
14.
Cereb Cortex ; 30(3): 1040-1055, 2020 03 14.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31403676

RESUMEN

The cortical code that underlies perception must enable subjects to perceive the world at time scales relevant for behavior. We find that mice can integrate visual stimuli very quickly (<100 ms) to reach plateau performance in an orientation discrimination task. To define features of cortical activity that underlie performance at these time scales, we measured single-unit responses in the mouse visual cortex at time scales relevant to this task. In contrast to high-contrast stimuli of longer duration, which elicit reliable activity in individual neurons, stimuli at the threshold of perception elicit extremely sparse and unreliable responses in the primary visual cortex such that the activity of individual neurons does not reliably report orientation. Integrating information across neurons, however, quickly improves performance. Using a linear decoding model, we estimate that integrating information over 50-100 neurons is sufficient to account for behavioral performance. Thus, at the limits of visual perception, the visual system integrates information encoded in the probabilistic firing of unreliable single units to generate reliable behavior.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Neuronas/fisiología , Corteza Visual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino , Ratones , Estimulación Luminosa , Psicometría
15.
PeerJ ; 8: e10551, 2020.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33384901

RESUMEN

Anthropogenic noise is a widespread pollutant that has received considerable recent attention. While alarming effects on wildlife have been documented, we have limited understanding of the perceptual mechanisms of noise disturbance, which are required to understand potential mitigation measures. Likewise, individual differences in response to noise (especially via perceptual mechanisms) are likely widespread, but lacking in empirical data. Here we use the echolocating bat Phyllostomus discolor, a trained discrimination task, and experimental noise playback to explicitly test perceptual mechanisms of noise disturbance. We demonstrate high individual variability in response to noise treatments and evidence for multiple perceptual mechanisms. Additionally, we highlight that only some individuals were able to cope with noise, while others were not. We tested for changes in echolocation call duration, amplitude, and peak frequency as possible ways of coping with noise. Although all bats strongly increased call amplitude and showed additional minor changes in call duration and frequency, these changes could not explain the differences in coping and non-coping individuals. Our understanding of noise disturbance needs to become more mechanistic and individualistic as research knowledge is transformed into policy changes and conservation action.

16.
Cogn Res Princ Implic ; 4(1): 43, 2019 Nov 21.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31748844

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Recent experimental work has shown that hyper-realistic face masks can pass for real faces during live viewing. However, live viewing embeds the perceptual task (mask detection) in a powerful social context that may influence respondents' behaviour. To remove this social context, we assessed viewers' ability to distinguish photos of hyper-realistic masks from photos of real faces in a computerised two-alternative forced choice (2AFC) procedure. RESULTS: In experiment 1 (N = 120), we observed an error rate of 33% when viewing time was restricted to 500 ms. In experiment 2 (N = 120), we observed an error rate of 20% when viewing time was unlimited. In both experiments we saw a significant performance cost for other-race comparisons relative to own-race comparisons. CONCLUSIONS: We conclude that viewers could not reliably distinguish hyper-realistic face masks from real faces in photographic presentations. As well as its theoretical interest, failure to detect synthetic faces has important implications for security and crime prevention, which often rely on facial appearance and personal identity being related.

17.
Phys Med ; 64: 89-97, 2019 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-31515040

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: To evaluate the feasibility of spatio-temporal generalisation of mathematical methods for protocol optimisation in interventional radiology. MATERIALS AND METHODS: Two model observers were considered:Furthermore, Low Contrast Detectability (LCD) was evaluated with a generalised statistical method by taking into account the noise integration capability of the human eye. A series of two alternative force choices (2AFC) experiments performed by four observers were used to evaluate the reliability of the proposed models. The evaluation of the mathematical methods was performed by comparing their results to the human observer performances in two steps: 1. Firstly, a series of simulated images were used to tune the models 2. In the second phase, tuned models were applied both to simulated images and actual images obtained with a commercial phantom to evaluate detectability scores. RESULTS: Evaluation with simulated images shows a good agreement with 2AFC results (RMSE < 10%). Phantom-based evaluations show a general decrease of such agreement, characterized by an RMSE lower than 16%. CONCLUSIONS: The agreement with human observer experiments supports the feasibility of the proposed generalisations. Thus, they could be introduced in quality control programmes for a deeper protocol-characterisation or for clinical protocol-optimization when dynamic images are involved.


Asunto(s)
Angiografía , Relación Señal-Ruido , Estudios de Factibilidad , Procesamiento de Imagen Asistido por Computador , Fantasmas de Imagen
18.
J Food Sci ; 84(3): 606-612, 2019 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30775783

RESUMEN

The power of discrimination tests is crucial in determining sample size and resources needed for testing. Although research has been conducted on the power analysis of several discrimination testing methods, much of the previous research has focused on basic taste solutions, which may not be directly applicable to food and beverage systems. The objective of the current study was to compare the power of seven discrimination tests: Panelist-Articulated-2-Alternative Forced Choice (PA-2-AFC), triangle, triangle with partial presentation, duo-trio, duo-trio with partial presentation, 4-category rating methods for R-index measure, and same-different pairwise comparison for R-index measure using commercial-type beverage products. Sixty-one prescreened panelists participated in the study. Six product comparisons were performed using tea, tomato juice (three comparisons), citrus-flavored carbonated soda, and cola-flavored carbonated soda. The tests were randomized over two testing sessions for each product comparison. Triangle testing methodologies were found to be overall the most powerful methods across product categories. The PA-2-AFC method was found to be the least powerful across all products. Thurstonian modeling predicts that the PA-2-AFC method would be the most powerful method contrary to the findings of the current study. The products tested were complex in both basic formulations and in changes made between control and variant samples. Complexity of the products may have influenced the discriminability by the panelists using different discrimination tests. Further research should be conducted to characterize the specific influence of sample complexity on the power of discrimination methodology. PRACTICAL APPLICATION: There are several discrimination testing methods that can be selected when determining whether two products are significantly different. A method with high statistical power can allow researchers to save time and resources when addressing this question. The current research compares seven discrimination test methods in order to determine which method results in the highest power for several common commercial-type beverage products. The results from this study demonstrate deviations from Thurstonian model predictions of method power revealing the need to experiment with several methods using commercial-type products commonly tested within a business or research setting prior to selecting an optimal method to use.


Asunto(s)
Bebidas/análisis , Gusto , Adulto , Humanos
19.
Behav Brain Res ; 359: 190-196, 2019 02 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30412739

RESUMEN

As a model organism, zebrafish have much to offer neuroscientific research and they are increasingly being used in behavioral neuroscience, for example to study the genetics of learning and memory. As fish are often considered "less clever" than mammals, it is important to understand how they learn and to establish optimal testing conditions. In this study, we compared the efficacy of food reinforcement and social stimuli in supporting Pavlovian conditioning, Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer, and acquisition of a two-alternative forced choice visual discrimination. Although equally effective in conditioning and in motivating discrimination learning, fish responded with shorter latencies when they were anticipating food but responded for a greater number of trials when anticipating the social stimulus. After learning, the reward was changed: food-reinforcement was replaced with the social stimulus and vice versa. Performance accuracy did not change, but response latency did: the group previously rewarded with food, but now rewarded with the social stimulus, showed a decrease in response vigor. This is a negative contrast effect, which is well established in rats, but was thought to be absent in fish because they lacked goal representation. Our results show that zebrafish, like rats, do have goal representations. Furthermore, we have shown that whereas food has greater incentive salience than social stimuli, fish become satiated rapidly, but motivation to seek social stimuli is sustained. We conclude that zebrafish are well motivated by a mixed economy of social stimuli and food.


Asunto(s)
Alimentos , Motivación , Conducta Social , Percepción Visual , Pez Cebra , Animales , Anticipación Psicológica , Atención , Conducta de Elección , Condicionamiento Psicológico , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Función Ejecutiva , Femenino , Objetivos , Masculino , Pruebas Psicológicas , Distribución Aleatoria , Recompensa , Saciedad
20.
Front Psychol ; 9: 2464, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30564183

RESUMEN

An important yet unresolved question is whether or not metacognition consists of domain-general or domain-specific mechanisms. While most studies on this topic suggest a dissociation between metacognitive abilities at the neural level, there are inconsistent reports at the behavioral level. Specifically, while McCurdy et al. (2013) found a positive correlation between metacognitive efficiency for visual perception and memory, such correlation was not observed in Baird et al. (2013). One possible explanation for this discrepancy is that the former included two-alternative-forced choice (2AFC) judgments in both their visual and memory tasks, whereas the latter used 2AFC for one task and yes/no (YN) judgments for the other. To test the effect of task on cross-domain association in metacognitive efficiency, we conducted two online experiments to mirror McCurdy et al. (2013) and Baird et al. (2013) with considerable statistical power (n = 100), and replicated the main findings of both studies. The results suggest that the use of task could affect cross-domain association in metacognitive efficiency. In the third experiment with the same sample size, we used YN judgments for both tasks and did not find a significant cross-domain correlation in metacognitive efficiency. This suggests that the cross-domain correlation found in McCurdy et al. (2013) was not simply due to the same task being used for both domains, and the absence of cross-domain correlation in Baird et al. (2013) might be due to the use of YN judgments. Our results highlight the importance of avoiding confusion between 2AFC and YN judgments in behavioral tasks for metacognitive research, which is a common problem in many behavioral studies.

SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA