Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Health Care Anal ; 30(3-4): 240-253, 2022 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36427133

RESUMEN

As embryonic stem cell research is commercialized, the stem cell debate may shift focus from concerns about embryo destruction to concerns about exploitation of the women who donate eggs and embryos for research. Uncomfortable with the polarization of the embryo debate, this paper proposes a more "contemplative" approach than intellectual debate to concerns about exploitation. After examining pitfalls of rigid intellectual positions on exploitation, the paper investigates the possibility of a broader understanding of donation for research where patients are seen as the intended beneficiaries of the donation. Together with other actors, research is perceived as mediating altruistic gift relationships that extend from donors to patients. The paper explores how this broader perspective on "donation for research" can open up new possibilities of understanding donation and addressing risks of exploitation.


Asunto(s)
Investigación con Células Madre , Donantes de Tejidos , Humanos , Femenino , Altruismo
2.
BMC Med Ethics ; 21(1): 30, 2020 04 25.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-32334575

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Preconception Expanded Carrier Screening (ECS) is a genetic test offered to a general population or to couples who have no known risk of recessive and X-linked genetic diseases and are interested in becoming parents. A test may screen for carrier status of several autosomal recessive diseases at one go. Such a program has been piloted in the Netherlands and may become a reality in more European countries in the future. The ethical rationale for such tests is that they enhance reproductive autonomy. The dominant conception of autonomy is individual-based. However, at the clinic, people deciding on preconception ECS will be counselled together and are expected to make a joint decision, as a couple. The aim of the present study was to develop an understanding of autonomous decisions made by couples in the context of reproductive technologies in general and of preconception ECS in particular. Further, to shed light on what occurs in reproductive clinics and suggest concrete implications for healthcare professionals. MAIN TEXT: Based on the shift in emphasis from individual autonomy to relational autonomy, a notion of couple autonomy was suggested and some features of this concept were outlined. First, that both partners are individually autonomous and that the decision is reached through a communicative process. In this process each partner should feel free to express his or her concerns and preferences, so no one partner dominates the discussion. Further, there should be adequate time for the couple to negotiate possible differences and conclude that the decision is right for them. The final decision should be reached through consensus of both partners without coercion, manipulation or miscommunication. Through concrete examples, the suggested notion of couple autonomy was applied to diverse clinical situations. CONCLUSIONS: A notion of couple autonomy can be fruitful for healthcare professionals by structuring their attention to and support of a couple who is required to make an autonomous joint decision concerning preconception ECS. A normative implication for healthcare staff is to allow the necessary time for decision-making and to promote a dialogue that can increase the power of the weaker part in a relationship.


Asunto(s)
Toma de Decisiones , Pruebas Genéticas , Padres , Autonomía Personal , Europa (Continente) , Femenino , Tamización de Portadores Genéticos , Humanos , Países Bajos
3.
Genet Med ; 21(10): 2381-2389, 2019 10.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30992550

RESUMEN

PURPOSE: This study aims to determine research participants' preferences for receiving genetic risk information when participating in a scientific study that uses genome sequencing. METHODS: A discrete choice experiment questionnaire was sent to 650 research participants (response rate 60.5%). Four attributes were selected for the questionnaire: type of disease, disease penetrance probability, preventive opportunity, and effectiveness of the preventive measure. Panel mixed logit models were used to determine attribute level estimates and the heterogeneity in preferences. Relative importance of the attribute and the predicted uptake for different information scenarios were calculated from the estimates. In addition, this study estimates predicted uptake for receiving genetic risk information in different scenarios. RESULTS: All characteristics influenced research participants' willingness to receive genetic risk information. The most important characteristic was the effectiveness of the preventive opportunity. Predicted uptake ranged between 28% and 98% depending on what preventive opportunities and levels of effectiveness were presented. CONCLUSION: Information about an effective preventive measure was most important for participants. They valued that attribute twice as much as the other attributes. Therefore, when there is an effective preventive measure, risk communication can be less concerned with the magnitude of the probability of developing disease.


Asunto(s)
Pruebas Genéticas/ética , Prioridad del Paciente/psicología , Prevención Primaria/ética , Adulto , Anciano , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Penetrancia , Factores de Riesgo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
4.
Patient Educ Couns ; 101(3): 422-427, 2018 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28947362

RESUMEN

OBJECTIVE: It is well known that research participants want to receive genetic risk information that is about high risks, serious diseases and potential preventive measures. The aim of this study was to explore, by qualitative means, something less well known: how do healthy research participants themselves make sense of genetic risk information? METHOD: A phenomenographic approach was chosen to explore research participants' understanding and assessment of genetic risk. We conducted four focus-group (N=16) interviews with participants in a research programme designed to identify biomarkers for cardiopulmonary disease. RESULTS: Among the research participants, we found four ways of understanding genetic risk: as a binary concept, as an explanation, as revealing who I am (knowledge of oneself) and as affecting life ahead. CONCLUSION: Research participants tend to understand genetic risk as a binary concept. This does not necessarily imply a misunderstanding of, or an irrational approach to, genetic risk. Rather, it may have a heuristic function in decision-making. PRACTICAL IMPLICATIONS: Risk communication may be enhanced by tailoring the communication to the participants' own lay conceptions. For example, researchers and counselors should address risk in binary terms, maybe looking out for how individual participants search for threshold figures.


Asunto(s)
Asesoramiento Genético/psicología , Genómica , Conocimientos, Actitudes y Práctica en Salud , Voluntarios Sanos , Medición de Riesgo , Anciano , Femenino , Grupos Focales , Pruebas Genéticas , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Investigación Cualitativa
5.
Bioethics ; 30(3): 203-9, 2016 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25908209

RESUMEN

Ethicists, regulators and researchers have struggled with the question of whether incidental findings in genomics studies should be disclosed to participants. In the ethical debate, a general consensus is that disclosed information should benefit participants. However, there is no agreement that genetic information will benefit participants, rather it may cause problems such as anxiety. One could get past this disagreement about disclosure of incidental findings by letting participants express their preferences in the consent form. We argue that this freedom of choice is problematic. In transferring the decision to participants, it is assumed that participants will understand what they decide about and that they will express what they truly want. However, psychological findings about people's reaction to probabilities and risk have been shown to involve both cognitive and emotional challenges. People change their attitude to risk depending on what is at stake. Their mood affects judgments and choices, and they over- and underestimate probabilities depending on whether they are low or high. Moreover, different framing of the options can steer people to a specific choice. Although it seems attractive to let participants express their preferences to incidental findings in the consent form, it is uncertain if this choice enables people to express what they truly prefer. In order to better understand the participants' preferences, we argue that future empirical work needs to confront the participant with the complexity of the uncertainty and the trade-offs that are connected with the uncertain predictive value of genetic risk information.


Asunto(s)
Conducta de Elección , Cognición , Revelación , Emociones , Libertad , Asesoramiento Genético , Hallazgos Incidentales , Consentimiento Informado , Afecto , Comprensión , Revelación/ética , Predisposición Genética a la Enfermedad , Pruebas Genéticas , Humanos , Consentimiento Informado/ética , Autonomía Personal , Probabilidad , Medición de Riesgo , Incertidumbre
6.
Eur J Hum Genet ; 22(4): 437-41, 2014 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24065111

RESUMEN

Incidental findings (IFs) are acknowledged to be among the most important ethical issues to consider in biobank research. Genome-wide association studies and disease-specific genetic research might reveal information about individual participants that are not related to the research purpose, but may be relevant to those participants' future health. In this article, we provide a synopsis of arguments for and against the disclosure of IFs in biobank research. We argue that arguments that do not distinguish between communications about pathogenic conditions and complex genetic risk for diseases fail, as preferences and decisions may be far more complex in the latter case. The principle of beneficence, for example, often supports the communication of incidentally discovered diseases, but if communication of risk is different, the beneficence of such communication is not equally evident. By conflating the latter form of communication with the former, the application of ethical principles to IFs in biobank research sometimes becomes too easy and frictionless. Current empirical surveys of people's desire to be informed about IFs do not provide sufficient guidance because they rely on the same notion of risk communication as a form of communication about actual health and disease. Differently designed empirical research and more reflection on biobank research and genetic risk information is required before ethical principles can be applied to support the adoption of a reasonable and comprehensive policy for handling IFs.


Asunto(s)
Bancos de Muestras Biológicas/legislación & jurisprudencia , Investigación Genética/legislación & jurisprudencia , Hallazgos Incidentales , Formulación de Políticas , Bancos de Muestras Biológicas/ética , Investigación Biomédica/ética , Investigación Biomédica/legislación & jurisprudencia , Investigación Empírica , Investigación Genética/ética , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo/ética , Estudio de Asociación del Genoma Completo/normas , Humanos , Factores de Riesgo
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA