Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 50
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Parasitology ; 142(7): 958-67, 2015 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25731985

RESUMEN

Cross-species infection among humans, chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and baboons (Papio spp.) is potentially a significant public health issue in Africa, and of concern in the conservation of P. troglodytes. However, to date, no statistical comparisons have been made between the prevalence, richness and composition of parasite communities in sympatric populations of baboons and P. troglodytes. We compared parasite communities in sympatric P. troglodytes and Papio papio living in a wilderness site, in the Republic of Senegal, West Africa. We asked whether, in the absence of humans, there are significant differences between these hosts in their interactions with gastrointestinal parasites. We tested whether host, location, or time of collection accounted for variation in prevalence, richness and community composition, and compared prevalence across six studies. We concluded that, despite being closely related, there are significant differences between these two hosts with respect to their parasite communities. At our study site, prevalence of Balantidium, Trichuris and Watsonius was higher in P. papio. Papio papio harboured more parasites per host, and we found evidence of a positive association between Trichuris and Balantidium in P. troglodytes but not P. papio.


Asunto(s)
Balantidiasis/veterinaria , Balantidium/aislamiento & purificación , Paramphistomatidae/aislamiento & purificación , Enfermedades de los Primates/epidemiología , Infecciones por Trematodos/veterinaria , Tricuriasis/veterinaria , Trichuris/aislamiento & purificación , Animales , Balantidiasis/epidemiología , Balantidiasis/parasitología , Balantidium/clasificación , Balantidium/fisiología , Heces/parasitología , Tracto Gastrointestinal/parasitología , Especificidad del Huésped , Pan troglodytes/parasitología , Papio/parasitología , Paramphistomatidae/clasificación , Paramphistomatidae/fisiología , Prevalencia , Enfermedades de los Primates/parasitología , Estaciones del Año , Senegal/epidemiología , Simpatría , Factores de Tiempo , Infecciones por Trematodos/epidemiología , Infecciones por Trematodos/parasitología , Tricuriasis/epidemiología , Tricuriasis/parasitología , Trichuris/clasificación , Trichuris/fisiología
2.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 368(1630): 20120422, 2013 Nov 19.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24101630

RESUMEN

The chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) is well-known in both nature and captivity as an impressive maker and user of tools, but recently the New Caledonian crow (Corvus moneduloides) has been championed as being equivalent or superior to the ape in elementary technology. I systematically compare the two taxa, going beyond simple presence/absence scoring of tool-using and -making types, on four more precise aspects of material culture: (i) types of associative technology (tools used in combination); (ii) modes of tool making; (iii) modes of tool use; and (iv) functions of tool use. I emphasize tool use in nature, when performance is habitual or customary, rather than in anecdotal or idiosyncratic. On all four measures, the ape shows more variety than does the corvid, especially in modes and functions that go beyond extractive foraging. However, more sustained field research is required on the crows before this contrast is conclusive.


Asunto(s)
Cuervos/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta , Animales
3.
J Hum Evol ; 61(4): 388-95, 2011 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21714986

RESUMEN

Savanna chimpanzees are known to re-use areas of the landscape for sleep, and patterns of chimpanzee sleeping site re-use are proposed as a referential model for early hominin archaeological site formation. We recorded the prevalence of deformed but healed branches and remnants of dead branches found around fresh nests at the savanna site of Issa in Ugalla, Tanzania. These old nest scars were found in 79% of 112 beds. We also randomly selected potential nesting locations for a subset of 32 beds within the same trees, and found nest scars in only 19% of these "control" locations. We then monitored 275 nests for up to 19 months for decay, regeneration of new branches, and re-use. Of these 275 nest locations, 24% were re-used within the first nine months of monitoring, and most re-use occurred when the nest had already decayed and was not easily visible from the ground. After 18 months, the proportion of specific nest positions re-used increased to 48%. This fidelity is likely a result of the creation of ideally-shaped support structures and supple new growth for mattress material with successive use of nest locations. We propose that specific nest site re-use may not be a direct product of environmental determination, but a result of "niche construction" through formation of good building sites within trees. Environmental modification through construction behaviour may have influenced both chimpanzee and early hominin ranging, and thus leaves behind recognisable patterns of artefact deposition across the landscape.


Asunto(s)
Comportamiento de Nidificación/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Animales , Arqueología , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Clima , Ecosistema , Tanzanía , Árboles
4.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 365(1556): 3267-76, 2010 Oct 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-20855301

RESUMEN

Modelling the behaviour of extinct hominins is essential in order to devise useful hypotheses of our species' evolutionary origins for testing in the palaeontological and archaeological records. One approach is to model the last common ancestor (LCA) of living apes and humans, based on current ethological and ecological knowledge of our closest living relations. Such referential modelling is based on rigorous, ongoing field studies of the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and the bonobo (Pan paniscus). This paper reviews recent findings from nature, focusing on those with direct implications for hominin evolution, e.g. apes, using elementary technology to access basic resources such as food and water, or sheltering in caves or bathing as thermoregulatory adaptations. I give preference to studies that directly address key issues, such as whether stone artefacts are detectible before the Oldowan, based on the percussive technology of hammer and anvil use by living apes. Detailed comparative studies of chimpanzees living in varied habitats, from rainforest to savannah, reveal that some behavioural patterns are universal (e.g. shelter construction), while others show marked (e.g. extractive foraging) or nuanced (e.g. courtship) cross-populational variation. These findings allow us to distinguish between retained, primitive traits of the LCA versus derived ones in the human lineage.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Hominidae/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Animales , Dieta , Ecosistema , Femenino , Hominidae/psicología , Humanos , Masculino , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Tecnología
5.
Am J Primatol ; 70(4): 402-9, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18246594

RESUMEN

Although much research has shown otherwise, chimpanzees are still often classed as rainforest-dwellers. Most long-term studies of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) are not situated in evergreen, closed-canopy equatorial forests, but instead are conducted in more open habitats. This study aims to elucidate the extent of chimpanzee ecological diversity by scrutinizing (recently) sympatric mammalian fauna at established study sites. We compiled presence or absence data on large mammal species at eight sites: Assirik, Bossou, Budongo, Gombe, Kibale, Lopé, Mahale, and Tai. The sites were rank ordered on the most basic ecological variable: annual total rainfall. Only three of the 65 mammalian genera compiled were sympatric with chimpanzees at all sites: Potamochoerus (bushpig), Syncerus (buffalo), and Panthera pardus (leopard). Some subfamilies (e.g. colobines) were present at most sites, but some families (e.g. hyenids) were absent at most sites. Some taxa (e.g. suids, cercopithecines) correlated better than others (e.g. canids) with basic ecological variables. The most extreme chimpanzee study site for which data are available is Assirik, Senegal. Nowhere else are chimpanzees sympatric with Erythrocebus, Alcelaphus, Hippotragus, and Ourebia. As chimpanzees are often behavioral models for extinct hominins, these living faunal assemblages have implications for paleo-ecological reconstructions of ancestral habitats.


Asunto(s)
Ecosistema , Mamíferos/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , África , Animales , Geografía , Lluvia , Especificidad de la Especie
6.
J Hum Evol ; 55(1): 48-59, 2008 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18275983

RESUMEN

Chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) predation on army ants (Dorylus, subgenus Anomma) is an impressive example of skillful use of elementary technology, and it has been suggested to reflect cultural differences among chimpanzee communities. Alternatively, the observed geographic diversity in army-ant-eating may represent local behavioral responses of the chimpanzees to the anti-predator traits of the army ant species present at the different sites. We examined assemblages of available prey species, their behavior and morphology, consumption by chimpanzees, techniques employed, and tool lengths at 14 sites in eastern, central, and western Africa. Where army ants are eaten, tool length and concomitant technique are a function of prey type. Epigaeically foraging species with aggressive workers that inflict painful bites are harvested with longer tools and usually by the "pull-through" technique; species foraging in leaf-litter with less aggressive workers that inflict less painful bites are harvested with short tools and by the "direct-mouthing" technique. However, prey species characteristics do not explain several differences in army-ant-eating between Bossou (Guinea) and Taï (Ivory Coast), where the same suite of prey species is available and is consumed. Moreover, the absence of army-ant-eating at five sites cannot be explained by the identity of available prey species, as all the species found at these sites are eaten elsewhere. We conclude that some of the observed variation in the predator-prey relationship of chimpanzees and army ants reflects environmental influences driven by the prey, while other variation is not linked to prey characteristics and may be solely sociocultural.


Asunto(s)
Hormigas/fisiología , Ecosistema , Conducta Alimentaria , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Conducta Predatoria , África , Animales , Hormigas/clasificación , Conducta Apetitiva , Geografía , Comportamiento del Uso de la Herramienta
7.
Am J Primatol ; 70(4): 393-401, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18161774

RESUMEN

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) make nests for resting and sleeping, which is unusual for anthropoid primates but common to all great apes. Arboreal nesting has been linked to predation pressure, but few studies have tested the adaptive nature of this behavior. We collected data at two chimpanzee study sites in southeastern Senegal that differed in predator presence to test the hypothesis that elevated sleeping platforms are adaptations for predator defense. At Assirik in the Parc National du Niokolo-Koba, chimpanzees face four species of large carnivore, whereas at Fongoli, outside national park boundaries, humans have exterminated almost all natural predators. We quantified the availability of vegetation at the two sites to test the alternative hypothesis that differences in nesting reflect differences in habitat structure. We also examined possible sex differences in nesting behavior, community demographic differences, seasonality and nest age differences as variables also potentially affecting nest characteristics and nesting behavior between the two sites. Chimpanzees at Fongoli nested at lower heights and farther apart than did chimpanzees at Assirik and sometimes made nests on the ground. The absence of predators outside of the national park may account for the differences in nest characteristics at the two sites, given the similarities in habitat structure between Fongoli and Assirik. However, Fongoli chimpanzees regularly build arboreal nests for sleeping, even under minimal predation pressure, and this requires explanation.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Biológica/fisiología , Ecosistema , Comportamiento de Nidificación/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Árboles , Animales , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Senegal
9.
Folia Primatol (Basel) ; 78(4): 240-4, 2007.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17505134

RESUMEN

We present the first indirect test of manually lateralized behaviour in non-human primates, based on wells dug for drinking water by wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Apes at Toro-Semliki Wildlife Reserve, in Uganda, dig bimanually in sandy riverbeds, leaving behind paired piles of excavated sand. The volumes of left- versus right-side piles do not differ, suggesting a lack of behavioural laterality, but this needs to be verified by further, direct observational data.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Agua Dulce , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Animales , Animales Salvajes
10.
Primates ; 48(1): 22-6, 2007 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17106789

RESUMEN

Right-dominant handedness is unique and universal in Homo sapiens, suggesting that it is a highly derived trait. Our nearest living relations, chimpanzees, show lateralised hand preference when using tools, but not when otherwise manipulating objects. We report the first contrary data, that is, non-lateralised tool-use, for ant fishing as done in the Mahale Mountains of Tanzania. Unlike nut cracking, termite fishing, and fruit pounding, as seen elsewhere, in which most individuals are either significantly or wholly left- or right-biassed, ant fishers are mostly ambilateral. The clue to this exception lies in arboreality; all other patterns of chimpanzee elementary technology are done on the ground. Arboreal tool use usually requires not only that one hand be used to hold the tool, but also that the other hand gives postural support. When the supporting hand is fatigued, then it must be relieved by the other. Terrestrial tool use entails no such trading off. To test the hypothesis, we compared frequency of hand changing with the incidence of major hand support, and found them to be significantly positively correlated. The evolutionary transition from arboreality to terrestriality may have been a key enabler for the origins of human laterality.


Asunto(s)
Hormigas , Conducta Alimentaria/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Conducta Predatoria/fisiología , Animales , Animales Salvajes/fisiología
11.
Primates ; 47(3): 279-83, 2006 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16604276

RESUMEN

The strength of the evidence for population-level handedness in the great apes is a topic of considerable debate, yet there have been few studies of handedness in orangutans. We conducted a study of manual lateralization in a captive group of eight orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) ranking the degrees of manual preference according to a defined framework. We analyzed five behavioral patterns: eat (one- and two-handed), make/modify tool, oral tool-use, and manual tool-use. Although some individuals showed significant manual preferences for one or more tasks, at the group-level both one-handed and two-handed eating, oral tool-use, and make/modify tool were ranked at level 1 (unlateralized). Manual tool-use was ranked at level 2, with four subjects demonstrating significant hand preferences, but no group-level bias to the right or left. Four subjects also showed hand specialization to the right or left across several tasks. These results are consistent with most previous studies of manual preference in orangutans. The emergence of manual lateralization in orangutans may relate to more complex manipulative tasks. We hypothesize that more challenging manual tasks elicit stronger hand preferences.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Animal/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Pongo pygmaeus/fisiología , Animales , Femenino , Masculino
12.
Folia Primatol (Basel) ; 76(4): 222-6, 2005.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16088189

RESUMEN

Use of flexible probes to fish for macrotermitine termites and manufactured wands to dip for doriline ants is reported for a new site of chimpanzee field study. The flexible probes of vegetation used in termite fishing and ant dipping by the chimpanzees of Fongoli, in southeastern Senegal, are similar to those used at Assirik (Senegal) and Gombe (Tanzania). Based on the principle that form reflects function, we predict that ant dipping when seen will prove to be the two-handed technique.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Alimentaria , Pan troglodytes , Animales , Hormigas , Isópteros , Pan troglodytes/psicología , Conducta Predatoria , Senegal
13.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 128(4): 840-5, 2005 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16110479

RESUMEN

There is much debate in behavioral primatology on the existence of population-level handedness in chimpanzees. The presence or absence of functional laterality in great apes may shed light on the origins of human handedness and on the evolution of cerebral asymmetry. The plasticity of long bone diaphyses in response to mechanical loading allows the functional interpretation of differences in cross-sectional geometric. While left-right asymmetry in upper limb diaphyseal morphology is a known property in human populations, it remains relatively unexplored in apes. We studied bilateral asymmetry in 64 skeletons of wild-caught chimpanzee using the humerus, second metacarpal, and femur. The total subperiosteal area (TA) of the diaphyses was measured at 40% of maximum humeral length and at the midshaft of the metacarpals and femora using external silicone molds. Overall, the TA values of the left humeri were significantly greater than the right, indicating directional asymmetry. This effect was even greater when the magnitude of difference in TA between each pair of humeri was compared. The right second metacarpals showed a tendency toward greater area than did the left, but this did not reach statistical significance. The lack of asymmetry in the femur serves as a lower limb control, and suggests that the upper limb results are not a product of fluctuating asymmetry. These findings imply behavioral laterality in upper limb function in chimpanzees, and suggest a complementary relationship between precision and power.


Asunto(s)
Fémur/anatomía & histología , Húmero/anatomía & histología , Huesos del Metacarpo/anatomía & histología , Pan troglodytes/anatomía & histología , Anatomía Transversal , Animales , Distribución de Chi-Cuadrado , Diáfisis/anatomía & histología
14.
Curr Biol ; 14(24): R1046-7, 2004 Dec 29.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15620636

RESUMEN

New findings from African rainforests show chimpanzees to have impressively advanced technology. They make tools of vegetation to harvest termites as in East and West Africa, but some apes in Central Africa show different techniques and tool sets geared for different tasks.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Alimentaria , Aprendizaje , Destreza Motora , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , África , Animales
15.
Am J Primatol ; 63(2): 87-93, 2004 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15195330

RESUMEN

Prolonged habituation times for wild great apes delay the collection of behavioral and environmental data, sometimes for years. However, genotyping of noninvasively collected feces can provide useful socioecological information in the meantime. We tested this premise on an unhabituated wild population of western chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus) at Mont Assirik, Senegal. Genotyping yielded information on kinship, group size, party size and composition, sex ratio, and ranging.


Asunto(s)
ADN/genética , Heces/química , Pan troglodytes/genética , Animales , Frecuencia de los Genes , Genotipo , Geografía , Repeticiones de Microsatélite/genética , Senegal , Razón de Masculinidad , Conducta Social
16.
Am J Primatol ; 58(1): 35-43, 2002 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12325117

RESUMEN

A survey of the western subspecies of chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes verus) was conducted from 1 February to 9 April 2000 in Sénégal, West Africa, by the Miami Assirik Pan Project (MAPP). In addition to the Assirik area of the Parc National du Niokolo Koba (PNNK), areas south and east of the park were surveyed. Nests made by chimpanzees were used to estimate chimpanzee distribution and densities. Within the PNNK, chimpanzees were estimated to occur at an average of 0.13 individuals/km(2). Chimpanzee nests were recorded in nine of 10 locales surveyed outside of the PNNK, as well as within the park. Data on 994 nests made by chimpanzees were recorded outside the PNNK, while 736 nests were recorded inside the park. Nest density in areas surveyed outside the PNNK, such as Bandafassi, Tomboronkoto, and Segou, was comparable to that of Assirik in habitats where nests were concentrated (i.e., evergreen gallery forest). The purpose of MAPP was to initiate long-term research of chimpanzees in southeastern Sénégal, as a follow-up to the Stirling African Primate Project (SAPP) of the 1970s. We sought to replicate the standards set by the SAPP project, except when technological innovations allowed improvement in data collection procedures (e.g., the global positioning system (GPS)).


Asunto(s)
Pan troglodytes , Reproducción , Animales , Recolección de Datos , Ambiente , Femenino , Masculino , Dinámica Poblacional , Senegal
17.
Am J Phys Anthropol ; 118(2): 184-90, 2002 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-12012370

RESUMEN

A host of ecological, anatomical, and physiological selective pressures are hypothesized to have played a role in the evolution of hominid bipedalism. A referential model, based on the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and bonobo (Pan paniscus), was used to test through experimental manipulation four hypotheses on the evolution of hominid bipedalism. The introduction of food piles (Carry hypothesis) increased locomotor bipedality in both species. Neither the introduction of branches (Display hypothesis) nor the construction of visual barriers (Vigilance hypothesis) altered bipedality in either species. Introduction of raised foraging structures (Forage hypothesis) increased postural bipedality in chimpanzees. These experimental manipulations provided support for carrying of portable objects and foraging on elevated food-items as plausible mechanisms that shaped bipedalism in hominids.


Asunto(s)
Evolución Biológica , Locomoción , Pan paniscus/fisiología , Pan troglodytes/fisiología , Animales , Conducta Alimentaria , Femenino , Masculino , Selección Genética
18.
Am J Primatol ; 54(4): 233-9, 2001 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11468753

RESUMEN

Of the living apes, the chimpanzee (Pan troglodytes) and bonobo (Pan paniscus) are often presented as possible models for the evolution of hominid bipedalism. Bipedality in matched pairs of captive bonobos and chimpanzees was analyzed to test hypotheses for the evolution of bipedalism, derived from a direct referential model. There was no overall species difference in rates of bipedal positional behavior, either postural or locomotory. The hominoid species differed in the function or use of bipedality, with bonobos showing more bipedality for carrying and vigilance, and chimpanzees showing more bipedality for display.


Asunto(s)
Locomoción , Pan paniscus , Pan troglodytes , Postura , Animales , Conducta Animal , Evolución Biológica
19.
Nature ; 409(6816): 12, 2001 Jan 04.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11343083
20.
Folia Primatol (Basel) ; 71(5): 334-41, 2000.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11093037

RESUMEN

Eight captive orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus) were given wooden blocks embedded with raisins and bamboo as raw material for tool making in a study of manual laterality. In about three quarters of the raisin extraction bouts, the orangutans held the tool in the lips or teeth rather than in their hands. Three adult males and 2 adult females showed extreme (> or =92%) preference for oral tool use, a subadult male and an adult female used oral tools about half the time, and 1 adult female preferred manual tool use. Most oral tool users made short tools (approx. 4-10 cm long) that were held in the lips and (probably) supported by the tongue. Preference for oral tool use does not correlate with body weight, age or sex, but it may be related to hand size or individual preference. This is the first report of customary oral tool use as the norm in captive orangutans; it resembles the behavioral patterns reported by van Schaik et al. and Fox et al. in nature.


Asunto(s)
Conducta Alimentaria , Destreza Motora , Pongo pygmaeus/fisiología , Animales , Fenómenos Biomecánicos , Conducta de Elección , Femenino , Masculino , Boca
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA