Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 56
Filtrar
1.
Intern Med J ; 42 Suppl 5: 44-6, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23035682

RESUMEN

There is no situation in medicine where outcome is so utterly dependent on time than in the treatment of patients with S-T elevation myocardial infarction. This life-threatening situation accounts for 30% of acute coronary syndromes. Prompt myocardial reperfusion saves lives, but health services need to be thoroughly organised to achieve this outcome. Unfortunately, a minority of patients in Australia present within 2 h of symptom onset and most patients receive reperfusion 3-4 h after. So health professionals begin at a disadvantage. A novel approach to this problem has been to give thrombolysis at first contact with the patient, before admission to hospital. A French study has assessed this practice against the gold standard treatment for S-T elevation myocardial infarction with some very interesting results. The implications of this study now challenge well-entrenched guidelines for the management of patients with this condition.


Asunto(s)
Servicios Médicos de Urgencia , Infarto del Miocardio/terapia , Intervención Coronaria Percutánea , Terapia Trombolítica , Ensayos Clínicos como Asunto/métodos , Servicios Médicos de Urgencia/métodos , Humanos , Infarto del Miocardio/diagnóstico , Infarto del Miocardio/fisiopatología , Intervención Coronaria Percutánea/métodos , Terapia Trombolítica/métodos , Resultado del Tratamiento
2.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 23(3): 306-16, 2001 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11404809

RESUMEN

Although paced serial addition (PSA) tasks are considered to be tests of general information-processing capacity, recent work suggests that performance on such tasks is influenced by arithmetic-specific variables. We designed two visual PSA experiments to determine whether the performance of normal adults would support predictions derived from the cognitive psychology of calculation. Experiment 1 showed that mixing familiar (Arabic numeral) and less familiar (Roman numeral) stimulus formats reduced scores below the averaged scores for pure Arabic and Roman lists. The Roman-Arabic order of addends was more difficult than the Arabic-Roman order. Experiment 2, which involved only Arabic numerals as addends, showed that performance could be impaired by constraining the trial-to-trial variability of sums. The results of both experiments confirm the importance of arithmetic-specific variables in PSA and provide support for an associative network model of calculation. In addition, the findings implicate interference from extraneous addends and responses as the performance-limiting factor.


Asunto(s)
Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Modelos Neurológicos , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
3.
Neuropsychology ; 15(2): 185-98, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11324862

RESUMEN

Children with congenital hydrocephalus, children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder, and normal controls were evaluated with measures of focused attention (Visual Orienting and Detection Task), sustained attention (continuous performance test), and attention shifting (Wisconsin Card Sorting Test). Components from these tasks have been linked to attention systems mediated by anterior or posterior brain networks. Children with congenital hydrocephalus showed an inability to focus and shift attention, which specifically implicated impairment of the disengage and move components of the posterior brain attention system. Children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder displayed the expected performance patterns on measures of focused attention once their difficulties with sustained attention were taken into account. However, they showed problems with shifting and sustaining attention, which are commonly associated with the anterior brain attention system.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/fisiopatología , Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiopatología , Hidrocefalia/fisiopatología , Logro , Adolescente , Trastorno por Déficit de Atención con Hiperactividad/diagnóstico , Encéfalo/cirugía , Derivaciones del Líquido Cefalorraquídeo , Niño , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Hidrocefalia/cirugía , Masculino , Tiempo de Reacción , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Campos Visuales/fisiología , Escalas de Wechsler
4.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 23(2): 137-48, 2001 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11309668

RESUMEN

The entire contents of six neuropsychology journals (161 volumes, 612 issues) were screened to identify dual-task laterality experiments. Of 112 experiments thus identified, 45.5% provided information about sex differences. Although 23 experiments yielded at least one significant main effect or interaction involving the sex factor, only 5 outcomes represented an unambiguous sex difference in laterality. All 5 of those sex differences support the hypothesis of greater hemispheric specialization in males. The confirmatory outcomes constitute less than 10% of the informative experiments and less than 5% of the total population of experiments. These data alone do not rule out the possibility that sex differences are chance findings (Type I errors). However, when considered along with similar results from perceptual laterality data previously examined, the dual-task data fit the pattern of a small but reliable population-level sex difference in human laterality.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Lateralidad Funcional , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adulto , Dominancia Cerebral , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuales
5.
J Int Neuropsychol Soc ; 6(5): 539-47, 2000 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10932473

RESUMEN

As noted by Satz, the prevalence of lateralized language in the general population is underestimated substantially by the proportion of people who show the typical asymmetry on a laterality task. In a series of two dichotic listening experiments with a total of 171 right-handers and 170 left-handers, we tested the hypothesis that increased reliability of measurement will lead to increased classification accuracy. Experiment 1 showed that neither the frequency nor magnitude of the right-ear advantage (REA) for fused rhyming words increased as the number of trials increased from 120 to 480. Ear-difference scores were highly reliable (r = .85), even when based on 120 trials. Experiment 2, which involved lists of dichotic word pairs, yielded similar results. Even though retest reliability of the ear-difference score for 132 word pairs was only .45, neither the incidence nor strength of the REA increased significantly when the number of pairs was increased to 528. The results indicate that the poor classification accuracy of dichotic listening tasks cannot be attributed to unreliability.


Asunto(s)
Pruebas de Audición Dicótica/normas , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Audición/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Prohibitinas , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Proyectos de Investigación
6.
Brain Lang ; 71(3): 373-90, 2000 Feb 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10716868

RESUMEN

Berlin et al. (1973) reported that either stimulus from a dichotic pair of consonant-vowel syllables is processed preferentially when its presentation is delayed by 30-60 ms. In the first of three experiments with 60 normal right-handed adults, we replicated the Berlin et al. "lag effect," but only for asynchronies between 60 and 90 ms. In Experiment 2 subjects focused attention selectively on one ear. The results indicated that focused attention and stimulus asynchrony have additive effects: Performance improved at the attended ear irrespective of stimulus asynchrony, but the lag effect remained unchanged relative to the divided-attention condition. Experiment 3 entailed a signal detection task that allowed separate analysis of detection and localization accuracy. As in previous studies, selective attention to one ear increased the accuracy of localization but not detection at the attended ear. Both dependent measures indicated a lag effect that remained invariant as attention was manipulated. These findings imply that the lag effect is attributable to a preattentional stage of auditory processing.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Pruebas de Audición Dicótica , Localización de Sonidos/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Fonética , Tiempo de Reacción , Percepción del Habla/fisiología , Percepción del Tiempo/fisiología
7.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 21(2): 265-78, 1999 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10515734

RESUMEN

In each of two dichotic listening experiments, 48 normal right-handed adults were instructed to attend selectively to the left and right ears and to divide attention equally between ears. Participants listened for specified targets and reported the ear of entry when the target was heard. Stimuli consisted of lists of digit names in Experiment I and lists of words in Experiment 2. Shifts of attention altered ear asymmetry for localizing but not for detecting digit names. For words, attention shifts altered both detection asymmetry and localization asymmetry, but the effect of attention on detection seemed to reflect differential retrieval from short-term memory rather than differential perception. In both experiments, shifting attention toward either ear resulted in a reporting bias such that signals were attributed to the attended ear more often than to the unattended ear. The results confirm our previous findings, for single pairs of stimuli, that volitional shifts of attention alter response selection rather than perception.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Percepción Auditiva , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Humanos , Masculino , Sesgo de Selección , Volición , Pruebas de Asociación de Palabras
8.
Nurs Res ; 48(5): 260-8, 1999.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10494910

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Although the behavioral changes with progressive dementia are seen to increasingly depend on the environmental context until late stage disease, measurement has not reflected this interaction in real time to allow examination of antecedents to disruptive behavior. OBJECTIVES: To develop and evaluate the psychometric properties of the Environment-Behavior Interaction Code (EBIC) for use in dementia care research with either sequential or nonsequential analyses of behavioral data. METHOD: Development of the computer-based (sequential event format) EBIC provided an observational coding system to classify all behavior and environmental context in real time, so that the probability of social environmental antecedents to resident disruptive behavior could be estimated. A checklist (interval format) EBIC, based on the same behavioral taxonomy, was developed for clinical outcome research. A total of 158 elderly residents of dementia care units were purposively selected from three large long-term care facilities for the psychometric study components. RESULTS: Psychometric results indicated significant (p < 0.01) known-groups validity for the disruptive behavior construct, which was defined as a composite of aversive, harmful, and high intensity neutral behavior. Interrater agreement for the event format of the EBIC was estimated by average kappa (0.65) and percentage agreement (78%). For the interval format, the mean interrater kappa was 0.80 with 96% agreement. Stability of the event format using a 2-week retest interval ranged from r= .50 (positive behavior) to r = 0.73 (negative behavior, defined as aversive + harmful). On replication with a new sample, stability was higher for positive (r = 0.92) and negative (r = 0.95) components, and for composite scores of nondisruptive (positive + low intensity neutral; r = 0.65) and disruptive (r= 0.85) behavior. CONCLUSION: This research provided support for the reliability and validity of both event and interval EBIC formats. Measurement using the EBIC taxonomy has applicability to dementia care research questions that call for either sequential analysis of social interactions or nonsequential analysis of behavioral outcomes in intervention studies.


Asunto(s)
Conducta , Demencia/psicología , Ambiente , Cuidados a Largo Plazo , Evaluación de Resultado en la Atención de Salud/estadística & datos numéricos , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Psicometría , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
9.
Percept Psychophys ; 61(5): 798-809, 1999 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10498996

RESUMEN

In a series of four experiments using rapid serial visual presentations of two target letters embedded in numeral distractors, with different numbers of display positions and with or without masking, we show that (1) the nonmonotonic, U-shaped attentional blink (AB) function, which occurs when all items are presented at the same display location, is eliminated in favor of a monotonic function when targets and distractors are presented randomly dispersed over four or nine adjacent positions; (2) the AB monotonicity is maintained with the spatially distributed presentation even when backward masks are used in all possible stimulus positions and when the location of the next item in sequence is predictable; and (3) the U-shaped AB is not due to position-specific forward or backward masking effects occurring at early levels of visual processing. We tentatively conclude that the U-shaped AB is primarily a function of the interruption of late visual processing produced when the item following the first target occurs at the same location. In order for the AB to severely disrupt performance, the item following the first target must be presented at the same location as the target so that it can serve both as a distractor and as a mask interrupting of interfering with subsequent visual processing.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Parpadeo/fisiología , Enmascaramiento Perceptual/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Sensibilidad y Especificidad
10.
Neuropsychology ; 13(3): 404-14, 1999 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10447301

RESUMEN

In 2 dichotic listening experiments, 96 normal right-handed adults attended selectively to the left and right ear and divided their attention equally between both ears. Participants listened for specified targets and reported the ear of entry. The material consisted of pairs of consonant-vowel syllables in Experiment 1 and pairs of rhyming consonant-vowel-consonant words in Experiment 2. Both experiments yielded a right-ear advantage for detection and for localization. Attention instructions had no effect on detection. However, focusing attention on 1 ear increased the number of targets attributed to that ear while decreasing the number of targets attributed to the opposite ear. The dissociation between detection and localization indicates that volitional shifts of attention influence late (response selection) processes rather than early (stimulus identification) processes. Selective-listening effects can be accounted for by a 2-stage model in which a fixed input asymmetry is modulated by a biased selection of responses.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Pruebas de Audición Dicótica , Detección de Señal Psicológica , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Fonética , Sensibilidad y Especificidad
11.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 21(1): 17-28, 1999 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10420998

RESUMEN

The contents of six neuropsychology journals (161 volumes, 612 issues) were screened to identify tactile laterality experiments. Of 73 experiments identified, 40% provided information about sex differences. Seventeen experiments yielded a total of 18 sex differences, of which 4 could be interpreted in terms of the hypothesis that functional cerebral lateralization is more pronounced in males. All 4 interpretable outcomes (constituting 5.5% of the population of experiments and 13.8% of the informative experiments) were found to be consistent with the differential lateralization hypothesis. The results, in isolation, do not justify rejecting the null hypothesis. However, when considered in conjunction with findings for auditory and visual laterality studies, the present results are compatible with a weak population-level sex difference.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología , Adulto , Niño , Dominancia Cerebral/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Teóricos , Caracteres Sexuales
12.
Neuropsychologia ; 36(3): 209-16, 1998 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9622186

RESUMEN

Twelve patients who had undergone surgical correction for either unilateral or bilateral congenital hearing loss were given a postoperative dichotic listening test for consonant vowel-consonant (CVC) words. Four normal hearing control subjects were also tested. Patients were first given a test of subjective loudness, and the level of presentation of stimuli in the atretic ear was adjusted accordingly. In the dichotic condition all controls exhibited a right ear advantage, and all unilateral atresia patients showed an ear advantage favouring the non-atretic ear. Patients with bilateral atresia essentially exhibited no ear advantage. Patients had the same error rate as controls in the dichotic condition, which suggests that ear advantage was not due to increased errors in the atretic ear. For patients there was a significant relationship between ear advantage and both preoperative interaural asymmetry in hearing loss and age at operation. Results suggest that a sensitive and critical period for development of the ear is complete by 5 years of age. Results also suggest some limited ability to adjust to permanent change in stimulation levels until, but not after, puberty.


Asunto(s)
Pruebas de Audición Dicótica , Pérdida Auditiva/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Niño , Desarrollo Infantil , Oído/crecimiento & desarrollo , Oído/fisiopatología , Oído/cirugía , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional , Pérdida Auditiva/congénito , Pérdida Auditiva/cirugía , Pérdida Auditiva Bilateral/congénito , Pérdida Auditiva Bilateral/fisiopatología , Pérdida Auditiva Bilateral/cirugía , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
13.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 20(4): 463-72, 1998 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9892050

RESUMEN

Tests of paced serial addition are used to detect diminished information-processing capacity. Nonetheless, performance might be influenced by modality-specific interference or by variables that specifically affect numerical processing. In a series of three experiments with normal adults, we manipulated, respectively, the modality in which addends were presented, the modality in which responses were produced, and the format in which visual addends were displayed. Performance was enhanced when stimuli were presented visually and when responses were made manually. When visual addends were used, Arabic numerals were processed more effectively than number words. Thus, performance was influenced by modality-specific interference and by presentation format. We conclude that paced serial addition tasks may not provide a pure measure of general information-processing capacity.


Asunto(s)
Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Estimulación Acústica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Matemática , Estimulación Luminosa , Valores de Referencia , Caracteres Sexuales , Factores de Tiempo , Escritura
14.
J Learn Disabil ; 30(5): 521-33, 1997.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9293234

RESUMEN

A high incidence of learning disabilities (LD) has been reported in children with neurofibromatosis Type 1 (NF-1), and many children affected with this disease are thought to have a form of LD that is characterized by selective visuospatial and motor deficits. However, the evidence is subject to sampling biases and is limited by the clinical-inferential methods used to classify children into LD subtypes. In the present study, objective statistical methods were used to categorize LD in 105 children with NF-1 between the ages of 6 and 18 years. A cluster analysis of achievement test scores yielded 10 groups; 6 of which met our criterion for academic deficiency. An analysis of neuropsychological data for 72 children with academic deficiencies with complete neuropsychological data yielded three groups: a neuropsychologically normal group (n = 28), a group with general academic deficiencies (n = 34), and a group with visuospatial-construction deficiencies (n = 10). The low incidence of visuospatial-constructional deficits and the absence of cases involving pure linguistic deficits is notable.


Asunto(s)
Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/genética , Neurofibromatosis 1/genética , Logro , Adolescente , Niño , Análisis por Conglomerados , Femenino , Humanos , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/clasificación , Discapacidades para el Aprendizaje/diagnóstico , Masculino , Neurofibromatosis 1/clasificación , Neurofibromatosis 1/psicología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Psicometría , Trastornos Psicomotores/clasificación , Trastornos Psicomotores/diagnóstico , Trastornos Psicomotores/genética
15.
Neuropsychologia ; 35(4): 457-69, 1997 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9106274

RESUMEN

According to Kinsbourne's functional cerebral distance model, asymmetric interference between concurrent cognitive and manual tasks reflects the functional specialization of the cerebral hemispheres. However, alternative explanations include initial-values artifact (statistical bias model), motor asymmetry (manual dominance model), and a combination of functional cerebral distance and manual dominance (two-factor model). We evaluated the competing models in four experiments with right- and left-handed university students for whom manual dominance was in effect reversed by requiring the dominant hand to perform the more difficult manual task. The cognitive load of the nonmanual task was varied within each experiment. The results did not support any of the models but, instead, reflected only tradeoffs between manual and nonmanual performance. The primary implications for future research are that performance on both tasks must be measured, and each task must be sensitive to interference from the other task, if observed asymmetries are to be interpretable.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Masculino , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Modelos Psicológicos
16.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 19(2): 185-90, 1997 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9240478

RESUMEN

Webster (1988) described a means of using scattergrams to represent the manual and nonmanual data from dual-task laterality experiments in a composite analysis. We used Webster's method and the conventional approach (i.e., separate analyses) to analyze the results of an experiment in which 40 right-handed adults performed a verbal and a manual task concurrently. Whereas separate analyses yielded dissimilar outcomes for the two tasks, the scattergram analysis showed that overall interference was greater when the right hand performed the manual task than when the left hand performed the manual task. Advantages of the scattergram method for analyzing dual-task data are discussed.


Asunto(s)
Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Adulto , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Psicológicos , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología
17.
Arch Clin Neuropsychol ; 12(5): 483-9, 1997.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-14590678

RESUMEN

The ability to utilize color information was investigated in 12 patients with mild to moderate probable Alzheimer's Disease (DAT) and in 12 age- and gender-matched control subjects. All subjects underwent testing of visual acuity and color vision before being tested with a cognitive task consisting of four conditions (no color, color as attention enhancer, color as valid cue, color as distracter). Although the groups did not differ in visual acuity or color vision, patients with DAT were less accurate than controls in all four conditions of the cognitive task. Both groups performed best with color as a valid cue and worst with color as distracter, but condition had a significantly stronger effect on patients than on controls. It is concluded that color is a potent stimulus attribute for patients with DAT.

18.
Cortex ; 32(2): 367-74, 1996 Jun.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8800622

RESUMEN

Dichotic consonant-vowel-consonant (CVC) syllables were presented to 96 right-handed children between the ages of 8 and 12 years. Children were assigned either to a "code" condition that entailed translating the CVCs into English words or to a "bird" condition in which the CVCs had to be matched to cartoons of birds. A differential ear asymmetry for the code and bird tasks developed linearly across four blocks of trials. By Block 4, the code task yielded a significant right-ear advantage and the bird task yielded no ear advantage. The results are inconsistent with any model that attributes ear asymmetries entirely to fixed structural characteristics of the nervous system. Instead, ear asymmetries are influenced by the subject's categorization of the stimuli, i.e., by "virtual stimuli". These appear to be constructed over time (blocks of trials).


Asunto(s)
Aprendizaje por Asociación/fisiología , Percepción Auditiva/fisiología , Oído/fisiología , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Niño , Pruebas de Audición Dicótica , Femenino , Humanos , Individualidad , Masculino , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas
19.
Brain Cogn ; 29(3): 307-25, 1995 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8838388

RESUMEN

In two experiments, 48 right-handed and 48 left-handed adults, respectively, performed speeded and consistent finger tapping with and without concurrent oral reading. Interference was measured in terms of change in tap-to-tap rate and variability. Experiment 1 confirmed a previous report that concurrent reading decreases the rate of speeded finger tapping and increases the rate of consistent tapping in right-handers, and that the right hand is affected more than the left. Experiment 2 showed that, for left-handers, concurrent reading decreases the rate of left-hand tapping more than right-hand tapping but increases the variability of the right hand more than the left. The double dissociation in left-handers between hand and dependent variable suggests that the speed and variability reflect different mechanisms of intertask interference. More generally, the findings illustrate the multidimensionality of motor performance and the risk of making inferences about neural organization on the basis of a single dependent measure.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Desempeño Psicomotor , Conducta Verbal , Adolescente , Adulto , Encéfalo/fisiología , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Lectura
20.
J Clin Exp Neuropsychol ; 17(4): 590-610, 1995 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7593478

RESUMEN

The contents of six neuropsychology journals (98 volumes, 368 issues) were screened to identify visual half-field (VHF) experiments. Of the 516 experiments identified, 42% provided information about sex differences. Sixty-eight experiments yielded a total of 92 sex differences, 23 of which met stringent criteria for sex differences in laterality. Of the 20 sex differences satisfying stringent criteria and lending themselves to interpretation in terms of the differential lateralization hypothesis, 17 supported the hypothesis of greater hemispheric specialization in males than in females. The 17 confirmatory outcomes represent 7.8% of the informative experiments. When less stringent criteria were invoked, 27 outcomes (12.3% of the informative experiments) were found to be consistent with the differential lateralization hypothesis. Six findings were contrary to the hypothesis. The results, which closely resemble results for auditory laterality studies, are compatible with a population-level sex difference that accounts for 1 to 2% of the variance in laterality.


Asunto(s)
Lateralidad Funcional , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas/estadística & datos numéricos , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Percepción Auditiva , Niño , Dominancia Cerebral , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Caracteres Sexuales
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA