Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 62
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Exp Brain Res ; 125(1): 43-9, 1999 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10100975

RESUMEN

In a previous study we investigated how the CNS combines simultaneous visual and proprioceptive information about the position of the finger. We found that localization of the index finger of a seen hand was more precise (a smaller variance) than could reasonably be expected from the precision of localization on the basis of vision only and proprioception only. This suggests that, in localizing the tip of the index finger of a seen hand, the CNS may make use of more information than proprioceptive information and visual information about the fingertip. In the present study we investigate whether this additional information stems from additional sources of sensory information. In experiment 1 we tested whether seeing an entire arm instead of only the fingertip gives rise to a more precise proprioceptive and/or visual localization of that fingertip. In experiment 2 we checked whether the presence of a structured visual environment leads to a more precise proprioceptive localization of the index finger of an unseen hand. In experiment 3 we investigated whether looking in the direction of the index finger of an unseen hand improves proprioceptive localization of that finger. We found no significant effect in any of the experiments. The results refute the hypothesis that the investigated effects can explain the previously reported very precise localization of a seen hand. This suggests that localization of a seen finger is based exclusively on proprioception and on vision of the finger. The results suggest that these sensory signals may contain more information than is described by the magnitude of their variances.


Asunto(s)
Dedos/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa , Corteza Somatosensorial/fisiología
2.
J Neurophysiol ; 81(3): 1355-64, 1999 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10085361

RESUMEN

To localize one's hand, i.e., to find out its position with respect to the body, humans may use proprioceptive information or visual information or both. It is still not known how the CNS combines simultaneous proprioceptive and visual information. In this study, we investigate in what position in a horizontal plane a hand is localized on the basis of simultaneous proprioceptive and visual information and compare this to the positions in which it is localized on the basis of proprioception only and vision only. Seated at a table, subjects matched target positions on the table top with their unseen left hand under the table. The experiment consisted of three series. In each of these series, the target positions were presented in three conditions: by vision only, by proprioception only, or by both vision and proprioception. In one of the three series, the visual information was veridical. In the other two, it was modified by prisms that displaced the visual field to the left and to the right, respectively. The results show that the mean of the positions indicated in the condition with both vision and proprioception generally lies off the straight line through the means of the other two conditions. In most cases the mean lies on the side predicted by a model describing the integration of multisensory information. According to this model, the visual information and the proprioceptive information are weighted with direction-dependent weights, the weights being related to the direction-dependent precision of the information in such a way that the available information is used very efficiently. Because the proposed model also can explain the unexpectedly small sizes of the variable errors in the localization of a seen hand that were reported earlier, there is strong evidence to support this model. The results imply that the CNS has knowledge about the direction-dependent precision of the proprioceptive and visual information.


Asunto(s)
Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Modelos Neurológicos , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Propiocepción/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad
3.
Perception ; 28(10): 1265-80, 1999.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10694974

RESUMEN

A study is reported of an exocentric pointing task in all three dimensions, in near space, with only two visible luminous objects--a pointer and a target. The task of the subject was to aim a pointer at a target. The results clearly show that visual space is not isotropic, since every set direction appeared to consist of two independent components--one in the projection onto a frontoparallel plane (tilt), the other in depth (slant). The tilt component shows a general trend across subjects, an oblique effect, and can be judged monocularly. The slant component is symmetrical in the mid-sagittal plane, requires the use of binocular information, and shows considerable differences between subjects. These differences seem to depend on the amount of binocular information used by each subject. There was a remarkably high level of consistency in the exocentric pointing, despite the absence of environmental cues. The within-subject consistency in the settings of the pointer corresponds to a consistency of about 1 min of arc in disparity of its tip, even though the pointer and target are separated by more than 5 deg.


Asunto(s)
Percepción Espacial , Adolescente , Adulto , Percepción de Distancia , Femenino , Percepción de Forma , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Psicológicas , Visión Monocular
4.
Network ; 9(3): 345-62, 1998 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9861995

RESUMEN

We perform a quantitative analysis of information processing in a simple neural network model with recurrent inhibition. We postulate that both excitatory and inhibitory synapses continually adapt according to the following Hebbian-type rules: for excitatory synapses correlated pre- and post-synaptic activity induces enhanced excitation; for inhibitory synapses it induces enhanced inhibition. Following synaptic equilibration in unsupervised learning processes, the model is found to perform a novel type of principal-component analysis which involves filtering and decorrelation. In the light of these results we discuss the possible role of the granule-/Golgi-cell subnetwork in the cerebellum.


Asunto(s)
Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Inhibición Neural/fisiología , Redes Neurales de la Computación , Adaptación Fisiológica/fisiología , Simulación por Computador , Teoría de la Información , Sinapsis/fisiología
5.
Exp Brain Res ; 122(4): 367-77, 1998 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-9827856

RESUMEN

The purpose of this study was to determine the precision of proprioceptive localization of the hand in humans. We derived spatial probability distributions which describe the precision of localization on the basis of three different sources of information: proprioceptive information about the left hand, proprioceptive information about the right hand, and visual information. In the experiment subjects were seated at a table and had to perform three different position-matching tasks. In each task, the position of a target and the position of an indicator were available in a different combination of two of these three sources of information. From the spatial distributions of indicated positions in these three conditions, we derived spatial probability distributions for proprioceptive localization of the two hands and for visual localization. For proprioception we found that localization in the radial direction with respect to the shoulder is more precise than localization in the azimuthal direction. The distributions for proprioceptive localization also suggest that hand positions closer to the shoulder are localized more precisely than positions further away. These patterns can be understood from the geometry of the arm. In addition, the variability in the indicated positions suggests that the shoulder and elbow angles are known to the central nervous system with a precision of 0.6-1.1 degrees. This is a considerably better precision than the values reported in studies on perception of these angles. This implies that joint angles, or quantities equivalent to them, are represented in the central nervous system more precisely than they are consciously perceived. For visual localization we found that localization in the azimuthal direction with respect to the cyclopean eye is more precise than localization in the radial direction. The precision of the perception of visual direction is of the order of 0.2-0.6 degrees.


Asunto(s)
Mano/inervación , Probabilidad , Propiocepción/fisiología , Percepción Espacial , Adolescente , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados
6.
Exp Brain Res ; 111(2): 253-61, 1996 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8891655

RESUMEN

To enable us to study how humans combine simultaneously present visual and proprioceptive position information, we had subjects perform a matching task. Seated at a table, they placed their left hand under the table concealing it from their gaze. They then had to match the proprioceptively perceived position of the left hand using only proprioceptive, only visual or both proprioceptive and visual information. We analysed the variance of the indicated positions in the various conditions. We compared the results with the predictions of a model in which simultaneously present visual and proprioceptive position information about the same object is integrated in the most effective way. The results are in disagreement with the model: the variance of the condition with both visual and proprioceptive information is smaller than expected from the variances of the other conditions. This means that the available information was integrated in a highly effective way. Furthermore, the results suggest that additional information was used. This information might have been visual information about body parts other than the fingertip or it might have been visual information about the environment.


Asunto(s)
Propiocepción/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Modelos Neurológicos , Probabilidad
7.
Percept Psychophys ; 58(4): 587-601, 1996 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8934689

RESUMEN

It has been found that the estimate of relative target direction is consistently biased. Relative target direction refers to the direction in which a target is located relative to another location in space (e.g., a starting position in the case of goal-directed movements). In this study, we have tested two models that could underlie this biased estimate. The first proposed model is based on a distorted internal representation of locations (i.e., we perceive a target at the "wrong" location). We call this the distorted location model. The second model is based on the idea that the derivation of target direction from spatial information about starting and target position is biased. We call this the biased direction model. These two models lead to different predictions of the deviations that occur when the distance between the starting position and the target position is increased. Since we know from previous studies that the initial direction of slow arm movements reflects the target direction estimate, we tested the two models by analyzing the initial direction of slow arm movements. The results show that the biased direction model can account for the biases we find in the target direction estimate for various target distances, whereas the distorted location model cannot. In two additional experiments, we explored this model further. The results show that the biases depend only on the orientation of the line through starting position and target position relative to the plane through longitudinal head or body axis and starting position. We conclude that the initial part of (slow) goal-directed arm movements is planned on the basis of a (biased) target direction estimate and not on the basis of a wrong internal representation of target location. This supports the hypothesis that we code displacements of our limbs in space as a vector.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Orientación , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Distorsión de la Percepción , Psicofísica
8.
Mov Disord ; 10(6): 761-77, 1995 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8749996

RESUMEN

This study describes the long-term deficits of a patient who, after a toxic encephalopathy, sustained extensive bilateral damage to both segments of the globus pallidus (GP) and the right substantia nigra (SN). There were no signs of lesions of the pyramidal tracts or of other motor structures. The most obvious deficits were an abnormal gait with an exaggerated knee extension and a tendency to fall slowly, especially when pushed backward. In contrast, Romberg's test on an unstable platform was normal, as were long-latency leg reflexes induced by perturbations. Inadequate anticipatory and compensatory postural responses, in particular across the hip and knee joints, and slow movements seemed responsible for the falls. Muscle tone was normal but reflex studies showed signs of abnormal facilitation and inhibition at various levels of the neuraxis. We conclude that the GP and SN lesions caused defective input to premotor cortical and brain stem target zones. Dysfunctioning of these zones leads to improper control of the descending ventromedial motor system responsible for locomotion, postural control, and reflex status. The deficits in upper extremity motor performance included delayed and slow movements, inaccurate amplitudes of ballistic responses, a lack of predictive control, and deficits in bimanual coordination. Sensory feedback, proprioceptive more than visual, played a powerful compensating role in rapid aiming movements. Regional blood flow (studied using 15(O)2) was reduced in multiple frontal cortical regions, among which are the hand areas of the supplementary and premotor cortex. We hypothesize that this reflected impaired functioning of these areas, caused by defective bilateral output from GP and SN, and resulting in the motor deficits of the arm and hand.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedades de los Ganglios Basales/inducido químicamente , Marcha/efectos de los fármacos , Globo Pálido/efectos de los fármacos , Dependencia de Heroína/complicaciones , Enfermedades Neuromusculares/inducido químicamente , Sustancia Negra/efectos de los fármacos , Adulto , Brazo/inervación , Enfermedades de los Ganglios Basales/diagnóstico , Enfermedades de los Ganglios Basales/fisiopatología , Mapeo Encefálico , Tronco Encefálico/efectos de los fármacos , Tronco Encefálico/fisiopatología , Femenino , Globo Pálido/fisiopatología , Mano/inervación , Dependencia de Heroína/fisiopatología , Humanos , Corteza Motora/efectos de los fármacos , Corteza Motora/fisiopatología , Vías Nerviosas/efectos de los fármacos , Vías Nerviosas/fisiopatología , Examen Neurológico , Enfermedades Neuromusculares/diagnóstico , Enfermedades Neuromusculares/fisiopatología , Reflejo Anormal/fisiología , Sustancia Negra/fisiopatología
9.
Biol Cybern ; 70(5): 417-25, 1994.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-8186302

RESUMEN

In this paper, we present a model for the development of connections between muscle afferents and motoneurones in the human spinal cord. The model consists of a limb with six muscles, one motoneurone pool, one pooled (Ia-like) afferent for each muscle and a central programme generator. The weights of the connections between the afferents and the motoneurone pools are adapted during centrally induced movements of the limb. The connections between the afferents and the motoneurone pools adapt in a hebbian way, using only local information present at the synapses. This neural network is tested in two examples of a limb with two degrees of freedom and six muscles. Despite the simplifications, the model predicts the pattern of autogenic and heterogenic monosynaptic reflexes quite realistically.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Neurológicos , Neuronas Motoras/fisiología , Músculos/inervación , Red Nerviosa/fisiología , Reflejo/fisiología , Médula Espinal/fisiología , Humanos , Matemática , Sinapsis/fisiología
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 99(3): 464-72, 1994.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-7957726

RESUMEN

In a previous study we found that the initial direction of slow, goal-directed arm movements deviates consistently from the direction of the actual straight line between the starting position and the target position. We now investigate whether these deviations are caused by imperfections or peculiarities in the processing of vision-related spatial information, such as retinal information, and eye- and head-position information. This could lead to incorrect localization of the target relative to the starting position. Subjects were seated in front of a horizontal surface and had to move their arm slowly and accurately in the direction of target positions. We varied the amount of vision-related spatial information. In experiment 1, subjects were presented with visual targets and could see their moving arm. In experiment 2, the subjects were again presented with visual targets, but now they could not see their moving arm. In experiment 3, the subjects were blindfolded and had to move their arm towards tactile targets. In all three experiments we found comparable consistent deviations in the initial movement direction. We also instructed congenitally and early-blind subjects to move their arm towards tactile targets. Their performance showed deviations congruous with those found in the sighted subjects, and possibly somewhat larger. We conclude that the deviations in the initial movement direction of slow, goal-directed arm movements are not primarily visually based. The deviations are generated after all spatial information has been integrated.


Asunto(s)
Brazo/fisiología , Ceguera/fisiopatología , Movimiento/fisiología , Visión Ocular/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología
11.
Dev Med Child Neurol ; 33(2): 118-29, 1991 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2015979

RESUMEN

Tracking performance was investigated in normal and clumsy children in two age-groups, six to seven and 10 to 11 years. Target signals moving unpredictably along a straight line had to be tracked, both with and without visual feedback. Performance was described in three ways: (1) performance in the low-frequency range (LF); (2) the delay between target signal and tracking movement (DL); and (3) a measure of tracking quality or over-all similarity in the shape of target signal and tracking movement (Q). Clumsy children in both age-groups had a lower tracking quality (Q) and longer delay (DL) than the normal children. Disturbances in the regulation of attention seemed to affect tracking performance, particularly of the six- to seven-year-old clumsy children. There was no significant difference between normal and clumsy children in the effect of visual feedback on tracking performance. This suggests that clumsiness is not linked to disturbance of integration of visual feedback information and motor processes.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Cinestesia , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Trastornos Psicomotores/psicología , Desempeño Psicomotor , Niño , Retroalimentación , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Destreza Motora , Orientación , Trastornos Psicomotores/diagnóstico , Privación Sensorial
12.
Exp Brain Res ; 84(2): 434-8, 1991.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2065750

RESUMEN

Information about the direction of the virtual line between two positions in space (directional information) is used in many decision-making and motor tasks. We investigated how accurately directional information is processed by the brain. Subjects performed two types of task. In both tasks they sat at a table. In the first task they had to move their hand slowly and accurately from an initial position 40 cm in from of them to visually presented targets at a distance of 30 cm from the initial position (movement task). We analysed the initial movement direction. In the second task subjects had to position pointers in the direction of the targets as accurately as they could (perceptive task). We found that in the movement task the subjects started the movements to most targets in a direction that deviated consistently from the direction of the straight line between initial position and target position. The maximum deviation ranged from 5-10 degrees for the various subjects. The mean standard deviation was 4 degrees. In the perceptive task the subjects positioned the pointer in similarly deviating directions. Furthermore, we found that the maximum deviation in the pointer direction depended on the length of the pointer: the smaller the pointer, the larger the consistent deviations in the pointer direction. The shortest pointer showed deviations comparable to the deviations found in the movement task. These findings suggest that the deviations in the two tasks stem from the same source.


Asunto(s)
Actividad Motora , Movimiento , Percepción Visual , Brazo , Humanos , Percepción , Estimulación Luminosa
13.
Dev Med Child Neurol ; 33(1): 40-54, 1991 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1995409

RESUMEN

The mechanisms underlying accuracy in fast goal-directed arm-movements were investigated in normal and clumsy children in two age-groups, six to seven and 10 to 11 years. Clumsy children in both age-groups had a longer movement time than normal children; this difference increased slightly when there was visual feedback. For both normal and clumsy children, the relative variability of the total distance moved was smaller than that of the distance moved during acceleration, indicating a variability reduction mechanism in the course of a movement. In the six- to seven-year-old group, the relative variability of the distance moved during acceleration and of the total distance was larger for clumsy than for normal children; this did not reach significance in the 10- to 11-year-old group. It is suggested that motor difficulties are linked to inaccuracy in open-loop control processes and to less efficient use of visual feedback.


Asunto(s)
Desarrollo Infantil/fisiología , Retroalimentación/fisiología , Trastornos Psicomotores/diagnóstico , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Factores de Edad , Brazo/fisiología , Atención/fisiología , Niño , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Destreza Motora/fisiología , Trastornos Psicomotores/fisiopatología , Proyectos de Investigación , Percepción Visual/fisiología
14.
Biol Cybern ; 65(4): 227-34, 1991.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-1932280

RESUMEN

A mathematical muscle model is presented that relates neural control signals linearly to muscle force without violating important known physiological constraints, such as the size-principle (Henneman and Mendell 1981) and non-linear twitch summation (Burke et al. 1976). This linearity implies that the neural control signals (defined as a weighted sum of activities in a nerve bundle) can be interpreted as the internal representation of total muscle force. The model allows for different relative contributions from the two force-grading mechanisms, i.e. the recruitment of motor units and the modulation of their firing frequency. It can therefore be applied to a variety of (distal and proximal) muscles. Furthermore, it permits simple mechanisms for controlling muscle force, e.g. in superposed motor tasks. The model confirms our intuitive notion that a weighted sum of activities in a nerve bundle can directly represent an external controlled variable, which in this case is exerted muscle force.


Asunto(s)
Modelos Neurológicos , Neuronas Motoras/fisiología , Músculos/inervación , Unión Neuromuscular/fisiología , Cibernética , Humanos , Matemática , Contracción Muscular , Músculos/fisiología , Sinapsis/fisiología
15.
J Mot Behav ; 22(3): 386-405, 1990 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15117666

RESUMEN

Tracking performance was investigated in children (aged 6-7 and 10-11) and in adult subjects. Target signals, moving unpredictably along a straight line, were tracked with the preferred arm, alternately with and without visual feedback. Qualitative observations indicate that tracking is based on continuous adjustments of the ongoing response to the continuously changing target position. No step-and-hold strategy could be detected in any of the three age groups. Tracking performance was described with four simple parameters, derived from linear systems analysis: (a) the delay between target signal and tracking movement (DL); (b) performance at the low-frequency range (LF), (c) performance at the high-frequency range (HF); and (d) a measure of tracking quality or overall similarity in the shape of target signal and tracking movement (Q). There was a considerable improvement in tracking performance with age, even after the age of 10-11, which was mainly demonstrated by a decrease in DL and increases in HF and Q. Tracking performance decreased only to a small extent when visual feedback was withdrawn. Age-related differences in the contribution of visual feedback to tracking performance could not be demonstrated.

16.
J Mot Behav ; 22(1): 67-84, 1990 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-15111281

RESUMEN

This study investigated how accuracy is attained in fast goal-directed arm movements. Subjects were instructed to make arm extension movements over three different distances in random order, with and without visual feedback. Target width was varied proportionally with distance. Movement time was kept as short as possible, but there were well-defined limits with respect to accuracy. There appeared to be a large relative variability (variation coefficient [VC]) in the initial acceleration. The VC in the distance the hand moved during the acceleration phase was much smaller. This reduction was accompanied by a strong negative correlation between the initial acceleration and the duration of the acceleration phase. Further, the VC in the total distance moved was less than the VC in the distance moved during acceleration. This result indicates asymmetry between the acceleration and the deceleration phase. This is confirmed by the negative correlation between the distance the hand moved during acceleration and the distance it moved during deceleration. Withdrawal of visual feedback had a significant effect on movement accuracy. No differences were found in the parameters of the acceleration phase in the two feedback conditions, however. our results point to the existence of a powerful variability compensating mechanism within the acceleration phase. This mechanism seems to be independent of visual feedback; this suggests that efferent information (efference copies) and/or proprioceptive information is/are responsible for the timing of agonist and antagonist activation. The asymmetry between the acceleration and deceleration phase contributes to a reduction in the relative variability in the total distance moved. The fact that the withdrawal of visual feedback affected movement variability only during the deceleration phase indicates that visual information is used in the adjustment of antagonist activity.

17.
Exp Brain Res ; 79(1): 138-42, 1990.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2311690

RESUMEN

Motor-unit activity in m. biceps brachii during isometric flexion contractions has been compared with motor-unit activity during a) slow voluntary movements against constant or increasing preloads and b) flexion contractions while movements were imposed by a torque motor. Recruitment levels and firing frequency behaviour of the motor units were found to be very similar when torques were generated during isometric contractions and during the imposed movements. However, these characteristics of the biceps motor units were quite different during the slow voluntary movements. It is suggested that the central activation of the alpha and/or gamma motoneurone pools of m. biceps brachii is different for force tasks and slow movement tasks, even if the same torques are exerted and/or movements are made.


Asunto(s)
Brazo/fisiología , Contracción Isométrica , Contracción Muscular , Músculos/fisiología , Conducción Nerviosa , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Reclutamiento Neurofisiológico , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Músculos/inervación
18.
Exp Brain Res ; 81(2): 303-12, 1990.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2397758

RESUMEN

Subjects made fast goal-directed elbow flexion movements against an inertial load. Target distance was 8 or 16 cm, randomly chosen. To exert a force in the direction of the movement subjects had to activate flexors of both shoulder and elbow, but shoulder flexors did not change appreciably in length during the movement. In 20% of the trials the inertial load was increased or decreased without knowledge of the subjects. Until 90-110 ms after the onset of the agonist muscle activity (about 65-85 ms after the start of movement) EMG activity was very similar in all conditions tested. The changes that occurred in the EMG from that moment on were effectively a later cessation of the agonist activity and a later start of the antagonist activity if the load was increased unexpectedly. If the load was reduced unexpectedly, the agonist activity ceased earlier and the antagonist activity began earlier. The latency at which EMGs started to change was the same for muscles around shoulder and elbow, for agonists and antagonists and for both distances. All adjustments had the same latency (37 ms) relative to the point where the angular velocity of the elbow in the unexpectedly loaded movements differed by 0.6 rad/s from the expected value. We discuss why simple reflex- or servo-mechanisms cannot account for the measured EMG changes. We conclude that appropriate adjustments of motor programmes for fast goal-directed arm movements start within 40 ms of the detection of misjudgment of load.


Asunto(s)
Brazo/fisiología , Movimiento , Músculos/fisiología , Esfuerzo Físico , Electromiografía , Humanos , Matemática , Modelos Teóricos , Músculos/inervación
19.
Exp Brain Res ; 81(3): 567-72, 1990.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2226689

RESUMEN

Motor-unit activity in m. biceps brachii, m. brachialis and m. brachioradialis during isometric contractions has been compared with motor-unit activity during slow voluntary (extension and flexion) movements made against external loads. During these slow movements the recruitment threshold of m. biceps motor units is considerably lower than it is during isometric contractions but the recruitment threshold of both m. brachialis and m. brachioradialis motor units is considerably higher. For all three elbow flexor muscles the motor-unit firing frequency seems to depend on the direction of movement: the firing frequency is higher during flexion movements (3 deg/s) and lower during extension movements (-3 deg/s) than during isometric contractions. The relative contribution of the biceps to the total exerted flexion torque during slow voluntary movements is estimated to increase from 36% to about 48% and that of the brachialis/brachioradialis is estimated to decrease from 57% to about 45% compared to the relative contribution of these muscles during isometric contractions. This difference in the relative contribution of the three major elbow flexor muscles is shown to be caused by differences in the central activation in force tasks and movement tasks.


Asunto(s)
Codo/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Electromiografía , Femenino , Humanos , Contracción Isométrica , Masculino , Neuronas Motoras/fisiología , Reclutamiento Neurofisiológico/fisiología
20.
Exp Brain Res ; 78(1): 139-46, 1989.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-2591508

RESUMEN

We have studied fast arm movements in response to double-step stimuli in two-dimensional space. In a previous paper we found that such movements did not start in the direction of the first or the second target, but in a direction between the two targets. The initial movement direction was found to depend in a continuous fashion on the inter-stimulus interval and on the reaction time. Therefore we concluded that the internal representation of a discrete target displacement is a gradually shifting internal target, moving from the first to the second target location. In this paper we investigate whether the arm movements also show a modification of the trajectory during the movement. An inter-stimulus interval of 100 ms was chosen, because then the initial movement direction is the same as in the response to a single-step displacement. We found that on average double-step trajectories deviate significantly from their original trajectory within 60 ms, and in some cases even within 30 ms of the start of the movement. We conclude that a motor programme is centrally modified according to a changed target location. We hypothesize that the generation of the motor programme starts after the target presentation, and that the activation levels for the appropriate muscles are continuously adjusted to move the hand in the direction of the current internal representation of the target.


Asunto(s)
Movimiento , Desempeño Psicomotor , Adulto , Brazo/fisiología , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA