Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 8 de 8
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Epilepsy Res ; 94(1-2): 69-74, 2011 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21324651

RESUMEN

This study provides evidence of non-verbal cognitive functioning in temporal (TLE) and frontal lobe epilepsy (FLE) patients by exploring the mechanisms and neural correlates of drawing abilities. Sixty-six patients with left (n=32) or right TLE (n=34), 30 patients with left (n=18) or right FLE (n=12), and 30 healthy subjects were compared. The Drawing from Memory (DfM) test required participants to design 16 living or non-living items; the total score was the sum of all scores blindly provided by three judges who had to identify the drawings. The verbal and visual Pyramid and Palm Trees Test (PPTT), Raven Colored Progressive Matrices (Raven CPM), Cube Analysis, Token Test, Word Fluency, Card Classification, and Rey Complex Figure Test Copy trial (Rey CFC) assessed different verbal and non-verbal functions. Non-parametric statistics indicated that, with respect to controls, both TLE groups and the left FLE patients had significantly lower DfM scores. In the TLE group, hierarchical regression analyses revealed that the DfM score was predicted by the Raven CPM and PPTT scores, while, in the FLE group, it was predicted by the Rey CFC. Thus, drawing abilities may be impaired by TLE or FLE owing to different mechanisms that involve semantic or executive abilities. Implying cooperation between temporal and frontal areas that support these functions, DfM may be a sensitive index of integrity of cortical areas or neural pathways damaged by focal epilepsy.


Asunto(s)
Trastornos del Conocimiento/etiología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/complicaciones , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/patología , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Femenino , Lateralidad Funcional/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Trastornos de la Memoria/clasificación , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Análisis de Regresión , Conducta Verbal/fisiología
2.
Behav Neurol ; 21(3): 155-63, 2009.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19996512

RESUMEN

Quality of life (QOL) is an important clinical endpoint, but it remarkably varies in patients with similar neurological conditions. This study explored the role of spirituality (i.e., the complex of personal transcendence, connectedness, purpose, and values) in determining QOL in chronic neurological disorders.~Seventy-two patients with epilepsy, brain tumours or ischemic or immune-mediate brain damage compiled inventories for QOL (WHOQOL 100), spirituality (Spiritual, Religious and Personal Beliefs, WHOSRPB), depression (Beck Depression Inventory, BDI), anxiety (State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, STAI), and cognitive self-efficacy (Multiple Ability Self-Report Questionnaire, MASQ) and underwent neuropsychological testing. With respect to 45 healthy controls, the patients reported worse QOL, with no difference between the four patient subgroups. Factor analyses of the WHOSRPB, STAI, and BDI scores and of the MASQ and neuropsychological test scores yielded four (Personal Meaning, Inner Energy, Awe and Openness, Mood) and three factors (Control Functions, Cognition, Memory), respectively. Mood, Cognition, Inner Energy, schooling, and subjective health status correlated with the WHOQOL scores, but at regression analysis only Mood and Inner Energy predicted QOL. This suggests that spirituality, as a personal dimension distinct from mood, contributes to determine QOL. A multidimensional assessment of QOL, including personal facets, may explain differences between patients with chronic neurological disorders.


Asunto(s)
Encefalopatías/psicología , Calidad de Vida , Adulto , Afecto , Isquemia Encefálica/psicología , Neoplasias Encefálicas/psicología , Enfermedad Crónica/psicología , Cognición , Epilepsia/psicología , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Pruebas Psicológicas , Análisis de Regresión , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Espiritualidad
3.
Epilepsy Res ; 84(2-3): 217-23, 2009 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-19261441

RESUMEN

This study determined the neuropsychological pattern of Unverricht-Lundborg disease (ULD) and its relationship to disease-related variables. Twenty-one ULD patients were evaluated using neuropsychological tests for abstract reasoning, attention, planning, set-shifting, theory of mind, visual perception, visuomotor coordination, ideomotor, orofacial and constructive praxis, language, learning, and memory. The control groups consisted of 21 healthy subjects and 21 patients with cryptogenic temporal lobe epilepsy (TLE). Multivariate analysis of variance showed that, in comparison with both control groups, the ULD patients showed significantly impaired abstract reasoning, attention, planning, word fluency, constructive praxis, and visuospatial memory and learning. Factor analysis of the test scores obtained by the ULD and TLE groups identified four factors (processing and execution, praxis, memory, theory of mind), and separate ANOVAs using epilepsy-related and demographic variables as covariates showed that the ULD patients had significantly impaired processing and execution. Correlation and regression analyses showed that processing and execution was significantly associated with diagnosis, disease duration and education, and logistic regression analyses indicated that it significantly predicted a diagnosis of ULD. To conclude, ULD is characterised by impaired processing and execution functions. This impairment is a strong independent predictor of ULD and may contribute to the characterisation of the diagnosis.


Asunto(s)
Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Síndrome de Unverricht-Lundborg/fisiopatología , Síndrome de Unverricht-Lundborg/psicología , Adulto , Anciano , Trastornos del Conocimiento/etiología , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Adulto Joven
4.
Epilepsy Behav ; 14(1): 261-8, 2009 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18930165

RESUMEN

Here we describe the unusual condition of selective semantic amnesia related to cryptogenic temporal lobe epilepsy. XY, an adult male patient, presented with partial seizures and disabling dysnomia. Neuropsychological tests revealed seriously impaired semantic memory. Electroencephalography documented ictal epileptic abnormalities in the left temporal lobe. Positron emission tomography showed reduced metabolism in the temporoparietal regions, but the results of magnetic resonance imaging and spectroscopy were normal. After 11 years, neuropsychological tests showed selective impairment of semantic memory and computed tomography provided normal results. This case shows that semantic memory may be selectively and lastingly altered, highlighting a distinction between semantic amnesia and global amnesia. Moreover, it is unique in that it occurred without evidence of gross temporal lobe pathology. The pathophysiological pattern of epileptic abnormalities in the left temporoparietal cortex supports the role played by dysfunctional neuronal networks (as provoked by focal epileptic discharges) in determining selective semantic amnesia.


Asunto(s)
Amnesia/etiología , Amnesia/psicología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/complicaciones , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/psicología , Epilepsia/complicaciones , Epilepsia/psicología , Corteza Cerebral/fisiología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/etiología , Trastornos del Conocimiento/psicología , Electroencefalografía , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Semántica , Adulto Joven
6.
Neuropsychologia ; 46(5): 1495-504, 2008 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18262206

RESUMEN

Although the pathological hallmarks of Alzheimer's disease (AD) and frontal variant frontotemporal dementia (fvFTD) predict different cognitive patterns, many comparative neuropsychological studies showed no difference in the expected cognitive domains. Inconsistencies in diagnostic criteria, small cohorts of patients, and neuropsychological assessment may account for such findings. Moreover, discrepancies in memory and executive dysfunctions that are expected to distinguish AD and fvFTD may reflect the basic brain organization. Adhering to a strict concordance of clinical and neuroradiological criteria, we compared many patients with AD and fvFTD using a large neuropsychological battery. One hundred and thirty-nine patients with AD (n=89) or fvFTD (n=50) were retrospectively considered in order to verify the diagnostic congruence of clinical and neuroradiological aspects. On this basis, 117 patients with AD (n=77) or fvFTD (n=40) with similar duration and severity of dementia were selected. Ninety-one healthy subjects were also controlled. Mean scores in tests for abstract reasoning, planning, set shifting, initiative, verbal fluency, immediate and episodic memory, constructive, ideomotor and orofacial praxis, selective and divided attention, visuomotor coordination, and visual perception were evaluated. Separate analyses of variance and post hoc Bonferroni tests showed that, with respect to controls, both patient groups were significantly impaired in all neuropsychological tests. Compared to fvFTD patients, AD patients were significantly impaired in episodic memory, selective attention, visual perception, visuomotor coordination, and constructive praxis, whereas no differences were found in executive, intellective, and linguistic abilities between the two patient groups. Logistic regression analyses revealed that episodic memory significantly predicted the diagnosis of AD while no executive deficit was able to predict the diagnosis of fvFTD. To conclude, memory, attention, and visuoconstructive deficits may distinguish AD with respect to fvFTD, in accordance with the severe temporo-parietal-occipital degeneration characterizing AD, but no executive impairment is consistently able to identify a relative compromise in fvFTD. Executive functions impairments possibly reflect the altered spatial-temporal integration of the frontal lobes with different brain areas, which prevents a clear-cut cognitive-brain correlation.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/diagnóstico , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Demencia/diagnóstico , Demencia/psicología , Anciano , Enfermedad de Alzheimer/patología , Atrofia , Atención/fisiología , Encéfalo/patología , Interpretación Estadística de Datos , Demencia/patología , Diagnóstico Diferencial , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Imagen por Resonancia Magnética , Masculino , Memoria/fisiología , Procesos Mentales/fisiología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Análisis de Regresión , Percepción Visual/fisiología
7.
Neuropsychol Rev ; 17(4): 455-76, 2007 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18026841

RESUMEN

Investigations of patients with temporal lobe epilepsy continue to result in significant advancements in the neuroscience of human memory, as they have for over 50 years. Recent reviews describing the state of the art in the clinical neuropsychology of TLE generally have emphasized (1) lateralization of cognitive deficits and the material-specific model of memory and/or (2) the relationships among pre- and post-surgery performance on standardized measures of anterograde memory, demographic and epilepsy variables and neuroimaging, neuropathology, and neurosurgery data. As information continues to accumulate about the pattern of performance on standard laboratory memory tests and the implications of these data before and after anterior temporal lobectomy for the treatment of TLE, innovative assessment techniques also are increasingly being applied. This review focuses on English language publications that addressed selected novel topics in adult TLE memory research. These topics are: (1) remote memory; (2) accelerated forgetting or long term amnesia; and (3) lateral versus mesial temporal lobe contributions to memory.


Asunto(s)
Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/complicaciones , Lateralidad Funcional , Trastornos de la Memoria/etiología , Memoria/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología , Adolescente , Adulto , Amnesia/complicaciones , Amnesia/fisiopatología , Amnesia/psicología , Niño , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/fisiopatología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/psicología , Epilepsia del Lóbulo Temporal/cirugía , Humanos , Trastornos de la Memoria/fisiopatología , Trastornos de la Memoria/psicología , Persona de Mediana Edad , Lóbulo Temporal/fisiología , Lóbulo Temporal/cirugía
8.
Epilepsy Behav ; 9(1): 133-9, 2006 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16707278

RESUMEN

People with epilepsy of comparable severity may differ widely in quality of life (QOL), suggesting a role for unexplored individual aspects. This study considered the possible role of spirituality. Thirty-two patients with focal epilepsy completed scales for QOL (World Heath Organization QOL, WHOQOL 100), spirituality (WHO Spirituality, Religiousness, and Personal Beliefs), depression, anxiety, and cognitive efficiency, as well as neuropsychological testing. The QOL and spirituality scales exhibited satisfactory internal consistency. Factor analyses of the scale and test scores yielded separate spiritual (Personal Meaning, Inner Energy, Awe and Transcendence, and Openness), affective (Mood), and cognitive (Cognition, Memory, and Perceived Cognitive Efficiency) factors. The total WHOQOL 100 score was significantly predicted by the Awe and Transcendence and Mood factors. The spiritual, Mood, and Cognition factors significantly predicted single QOL domains. These preliminary results highlight the contribution of spirituality to QOL in epilepsy, encouraging future studies. This could influence the conceptualization and assessment of QOL in these patients.


Asunto(s)
Epilepsias Parciales/psicología , Calidad de Vida , Espiritualidad , Adulto , Afecto , Cognición , Análisis Factorial , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Proyectos Piloto , Análisis de Regresión , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA