Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 33
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 85(6): 1768-1776, 2023 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37644344

RESUMEN

In the "classic" vanishing ball illusion (VBI), a magician pretends to throw a ball and the audience sees it go up and then disappear. To gain a better understanding of the mechanisms involved, we conducted two experiments to analyze the permeability of this type of illusion to auditory information. A modified version of the VBI (i.e., the Vanishing Coin Illusion [VCI]), was presented, and this was either accompanied by a sound or was not. The results show that the presence of a sound adds to the success of the illusion (Experiment 1), especially when this sound is congruent with the illusion (Experiment 2). Based on these results, we discuss the mechanisms at work in this illusion.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Percepción de Movimiento , Humanos , Percepción Visual , Sonido , Visión Ocular , Percepción Auditiva , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Estimulación Acústica
2.
Psychol Res ; 87(8): 2380-2389, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37071176

RESUMEN

Intuition plays a central role in cognition in general and expertise in particular. Dreyfus and Dreyfus's (1986) and Gobet and Chassy's (2008) theories of expert intuition propose that a characteristic feature of expert intuition is the holistic understanding displayed by experts. The ideal way to test this prediction is to use highly expert participants and short presentation times. Chess players (N = 63), ranging from candidate masters to world-class players, had to evaluate chess problems. Evaluating the problems required an understanding of the position as a whole. Results demonstrated an effect of skill (better players had better evaluations), complexity (simpler positions were better evaluated than complex positions) and balance (accuracy diminished when the true evaluations became more extreme). A regression analysis showed that skill accounted for 44% of the variance in evaluation error. These important results support the central role of holistic intuition in expertise.


Asunto(s)
Intuición , Deportes , Humanos , Cognición
3.
Mem Cognit ; 51(6): 1358-1373, 2023 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36715886

RESUMEN

The present study investigated interactions between iconic memory and long-term memory during visual search. The contextual cueing paradigm was combined with a brief presentation procedure to determine whether statistical learning takes place from flashed displays and acts as a cue to guide spatial attention within iconic memory in subsequent visual search. In five experiments, participants were exposed to search displays that were presented for only 50 ms and had to detect a target among 11 distractors. Half of those displays were repeated across the task and half were presented once. The results revealed a contextual cueing effect in four of the experiments, revealing the capacity of the human brain to extract spatial regularities from very brief visual presentations and to later activate this knowledge to guide attention within a representation briefly maintained within iconic memory.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Aprendizaje , Humanos , Tiempo de Reacción/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Memoria a Largo Plazo , Conocimiento
4.
Exp Psychol ; 70(6): 315-323, 2023 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38602115

RESUMEN

When we look at a picture, we tend to remember it by enriching the constructed mental representation with elements not present but probable outside the current view. The tendency to remember the perceived view with a broader scope is known as boundary extension (BE). Does BE benefit from paying reduced attention to the picture? While attention plays a central role in memory, only a few studies to date have investigated this question in the field of BE. In this research, participants completed a BE task in single- and dual-task conditions. The results indicate that BE is eliminated when the attention is divided on the onset of scene construction. We therefore discuss the role of attention in BE.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental , Percepción Visual , Humanos , Probabilidad
5.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 29(6): 2240-2246, 2022 Dec.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35879592

RESUMEN

While many studies have highlighted the existence of the anchoring effect in a wide variety of domains, no study to date has investigated its impact on memory. The present study aimed to test whether an irrelevant numerical anchor not only influences an estimate but also modifies the memory of the associated event. Two experiments (total N = 259) were conducted, combining the methodology used by Loftus and Palmer (Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 13, 585-589, 1974) and a classic anchoring paradigm. The results show that an irrelevant numerical anchor can modify the estimate of a car's speed and produce false memories of the event. We discuss the link between the processes underlying the anchoring effect and the false memory phenomenon.


Asunto(s)
Memoria , Aprendizaje Verbal , Humanos , Conducta Verbal , Represión Psicológica
6.
Q J Exp Psychol (Hove) ; 75(11): 2149-2158, 2022 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-34904457

RESUMEN

To facilitate our interactions with the surroundings, the human brain sometimes reshapes the situations that it faces to simplify them. This phenomenon has been widely studied in the context of reasoning, especially through the attribute substitution error. It has however been given much less attention in the field of perception. Recent research on the bat-and-ball problem suggests that reasoners are able to intuitively detect attribute substitution errors. Using a perceptual illusion drawn from the field of magic, we investigate the extent to which a perceptual form of attribute substitution depends on executive resources and can be detected. We also investigate the relationship between susceptibility to attribute substitution error in the flushtration count illusion and in a French adaptation of the bat-and-ball problem. Finally, we investigate the link between the intuitive cognitive style (assessed by the cognitive reflection test) and the susceptibility to the flushtration count illusion. Our results suggest that participants do not detect perceptual attribute substitution error, that this phenomenon could be independent of the executive resources allocated to the task, and could rest on mechanisms distinct from those that produce errors in reasoning. We discuss differences between these two phenomena, and factors that may explain them.


Asunto(s)
Quirópteros , Ilusiones , Animales , Atención , Humanos , Solución de Problemas , Pensamiento
7.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 80(8): 1932-1945, 2018 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30014317

RESUMEN

Boundary extension (BE) refers to the tendency to remember a previously perceived scene with a greater spatial expanse. This phenomenon is described as resulting from different sources of information: external (i.e., visual) and internally driven (i.e., amodal, conceptual, and contextual) information. Although the literature has emphasized the role of top-down expectations to account for layout extrapolation, their effect has rarely been tested experimentally. In this research, we attempted to determine how visual context affects BE, as a function of scene exposure duration (long, short). To induce knowledge about visual context, the memorization phase of the camera distance paradigm was preceded by a preexposure phase, during which each of the to-be-memorized scenes was presented in a larger spatial framework. In an initial experiment, we examined the effect of contextual knowledge with presentation duration, allowing for in-depth processing of visual information during encoding (i.e., 15 s). The results indicated that participants exposed to the preexposure showed decreased BE, and displayed no directional memory error in some conditions. Because the effect of context is known to occur at an early stage of scene perception, in a second experiment we sought to determine whether the effect of a preview occurs during the first fixation on a visual scene. The results indicated that BE seems not to be modulated by this factor at very brief presentation durations. These results are discussed in light of current visual scene representation theories.


Asunto(s)
Recuerdo Mental/fisiología , Percepción Espacial/fisiología , Memoria Espacial/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
8.
Br J Psychol ; 109(4): 850-861, 2018 Nov.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29665071

RESUMEN

When faced with a difficult question, people sometimes work out an answer to a related, easier question without realizing that a substitution has taken place (e.g., Kahneman, 2011, Thinking, fast and slow. New York, Farrar, Strauss, Giroux). In two experiments, we investigated whether this attribute substitution effect can also affect the interpretation of a simple visual event sequence. We used a magic trick called the 'Flushtration Count Illusion', which involves a technique used by magicians to give the illusion of having seen multiple cards with identical backs, when in fact only the back of one card (the bottom card) is repeatedly shown. In Experiment 1, we demonstrated that most participants are susceptible to the illusion, even if they have the visual and analytical reasoning capacity to correctly process the sequence. In Experiment 2, we demonstrated that participants construct a biased and simplified representation of the Flushtration Count by substituting some attributes of the event sequence. We discussed of the psychological processes underlying this attribute substitution effect.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adolescente , Femenino , Humanos , Magia , Masculino , Solución de Problemas , Pensamiento , Adulto Joven
9.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 80(6): 1420-1435, 2018 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29651752

RESUMEN

Since the seminal study by Chun and Jiang (Cognitive Psychology, 36, 28-71, 1998), a large body of research based on the contextual-cueing paradigm has shown that the cognitive system is capable of extracting statistical contingencies from visual environments. Most of these studies have focused on how individuals learn regularities found within an intratrial temporal window: A context predicts the target position within a given trial. However, Ono, Jiang, and Kawahara (Journal of Experimental Psychology, 31, 703-712, 2005) provided evidence of an intertrial implicit-learning effect when a distractor configuration in preceding trials N - 1 predicted the target location in trials N. The aim of the present study was to gain further insight into this effect by examining whether it occurs when predictive relationships are impeded by interfering task-relevant noise (Experiments 2 and 3) or by a long delay (Experiments 1, 4, and 5). Our results replicated the intertrial contextual-cueing effect, which occurred in the condition of temporally close contingencies. However, there was no evidence of integration across long-range spatiotemporal contingencies, suggesting a temporal limitation of statistical learning.


Asunto(s)
Señales (Psicología) , Aprendizaje , Estadística como Asunto , Percepción Visual , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo , Adulto Joven
10.
J Mot Behav ; 50(3): 268-274, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28850319

RESUMEN

Can Alzheimer's disease (AD) patients efficiently learn to perform a complex motor skill when relying on procedural knowledge? To address this question, the authors compared the golf-putting performance of AD patients, older adults, and younger adults in 2 different learning situations: one that promotes high error rates (thus increasing the reliance on declarative knowledge) or one that promotes low error rates (thus increasing the reliance on procedural knowledge). Motor performance was poorer overall for AD patients and older adults relative to younger adults in the high-error condition but equivalent between similar groups in the low-error condition. Also, AD patients in the low-error condition had better performance at the final putting distance relative to those in the high-error condition. This performance facilitation for AD patients likely stems from intact procedural knowledge.


Asunto(s)
Enfermedad de Alzheimer/psicología , Conocimiento , Aprendizaje , Destreza Motora , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Estudios de Casos y Controles , Femenino , Golf , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Joven
11.
Exp Aging Res ; 44(1): 82-93, 2018.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-29161195

RESUMEN

This commentary explores the relationships between the construct of successful aging and the experimental psychology of human aging-cognitive gerontology. What can or should cognitive gerontology contribute to understanding, defining, and assessing successful aging? Standards for successful aging reflect value judgments that are culturally and historically situated. Fundamentally, they address social policy; they are prescriptive. If individuals or groups are deemed to be aging successfully, then their characteristics or situations can be emulated. If an individual or a group is deemed to be aging unsuccessfully, then intervention should be considered. Although science is never culture-free or ahistorical, cognitive gerontology is primarily descriptive of age-related change. It is not prescriptive. It is argue that cognitive gerontology has little to contribute to setting standards for successful aging. If, however, better cognitive function is taken as a marker of more successful aging-something not universally accepted-then cognitive gerontology can play an important assessment role. It has a great deal to contribute in determining whether an individual or a group evidences better cognitive function than another. More importantly, cognitive gerontology can provide tools to evaluate the effects of interventions. It can provide targeted measures of perception, attention, memory, executive function, and other facets of cognition that are more sensitive to change than most clinical measures. From a deep understanding of factors affecting cognitive function, cognitive gerontology can also suggest possible interventions. A brief narrative review of interventions that have and have not led to improved cognitive function in older adults. Finally, the enormous range is addressed in the estimates of the proportion of the population that meets a standard for aging successfully, from less than 10% to more than 90%. For research purposes, it would be better to replace absolute cutoffs with correlational approaches (e.g., Freund & Baltes, 1998, Psychology and Aging, 13, 531-543). For policy purposes, cutoffs are necessary, but we propose that assessments of successful aging be based not on absolute cutoffs but on population proportions. An example of one possible standard is this: Those more than 1 standard deviation above the mean are aging successfully; those more than 1 standard deviation below the mean are aging unsuccessfully; those in between are aging usually. Adoption of such a standard may reduce the wide discrepancies in the incidence of successful aging reported in the literature.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Ciencia Cognitiva/métodos , Geriatría/métodos , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
12.
Iperception ; 8(5): 2041669517723652, 2017.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28932380

RESUMEN

In a constantly changing environment, one of the conditions for adaptation is based on the visual system's ability to realize predictions. In this context, a question that arises is the evolution of the processes allowing anticipation with regard to the acquisition of knowledge relative to specific situations. We sought to study this question by focusing on boundary extension, the tendency to overestimate the scope of a previously perceived scene. We presented to novice, beginner, and expert car drivers road scenes in the form of approach sequences constituting very briefly displayed photographs (i.e., 250 milliseconds each), in order to determine the effect of expertise at an early stage of scene perception. After three presentations, participants had to judge whether a fourth photograph was the same, closer up, or further away than the third one. When experts and beginners showed a classical boundary extension effect, novices presented no directional memory distortion. Different hypotheses are discussed.

13.
Br J Psychol ; 108(2): 259-275, 2017 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28369841

RESUMEN

Drivers face frequent distraction on the roadways, but little is known about situations placing them at risk of misallocating visual attention. To investigate this issue, we asked participants to search for a red target embedded within simulated driving scenes (photographs taken from inside a car) in three experiments. Distraction was induced by presenting, via a GPS unit, red or green distractors positioned in an irrelevant location at which the target never appeared. If the salient distractor captures attention, visual search should be slower on distractor-present trials than distractor-absent trials. In Experiment 1, salient distractors yielded no such capture effect. In Experiment 2, we decreased the frequency of the salient distractor from 50% of trials to only 10% or 20% of trials. Capture effects were almost five times larger for the 10% occurrence group than for the 20% occurrence group. In Experiment 3, the amount of available central resources was manipulated by asking participants to either simultaneously monitor or ignore a stream of spoken digits. Capture effects were much larger for the dual-task group than for the single-task group. In summary, these findings identify risk factors for attentional capture in real-world driving scenes: distractor rarity and diversion of attention.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Conducción Distraída , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto Joven
14.
Cognition ; 154: 169-173, 2016 09.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27318598

RESUMEN

In everyday life, several factors limit the human capacity to think differently. The present study shows that implanting an unlikely and unfamiliar idea in the mind can prevent participants from finding a more obvious one. To demonstrate this, we used a technique often adopted by magicians to misrepresent the method of a trick: the false solution. Our results reveal that a single exposure to an unlikely false solution (the magician can influence the spectator's choice with his gesture) before the presentation of a card trick can prevent participants from finding the real (more obvious) secret of a trick, even if they are invited to search for an alternative solution.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Solución de Problemas , Adulto , Conducta de Elección , Humanos , Adulto Joven
15.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 23(6): 1810-1817, 2016 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27184252

RESUMEN

In the vanishing ball illusion (VBI), the magician throws a ball up into the air twice and then pretends to do a third throw. On the third (fake) throw, the audience sees the ball go up and then disappear. In this article, we study the psychological mechanisms at play in this magic trick. We test the hypothesis that the illusion is based on representational momentum (RM), a psychological phenomenon in which the observer perceives the stopping point of a moving scene as being located farther ahead in the direction of motion than it really is. To determine whether the mechanisms involved in VBI are similar to those underlying RM, we compared the results of a standard VBI task to those obtained on an RM task designed to be very close to the VBI task. The results showed that VBI sensitivity was not associated with a higher anticipation score on the RM task. Unexpectedly, we found that participants who were sensitive to the illusion even obtained a weaker RM effect. We discuss several hypotheses that might account for these results.


Asunto(s)
Ilusiones , Percepción de Movimiento , Femenino , Humanos , Magia , Masculino , Percepción Visual , Adulto Joven
16.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 78(1): 21-9, 2016 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26676869

RESUMEN

In the vanishing ball illusion (VBI), a magician throws a ball up in the air twice, after which he pretends to toss it up again, when in fact it remains secretly concealed in his hand. Observers perceive an imaginary ball disappearing into the air. According to Kuhn and Land (2006), the VBI during the fake throw is mediated by the magician's gaze and/or head direction (also called "social cues") as he looks toward the imaginary ball. The aim of this article is to test an alternative interpretation. According to our hypothesis, the magician's social cues are not essential to the VBI. We compared the numbers of participants experiencing the VBI when the magician's social cues were directed toward the illusory ball and when the magician's social cues were either hidden behind a black mask (Exp. 1) or stationary (Exp. 2). The results showed that the number of observers experiencing the VBI was high (almost two-thirds of the participants), regardless of whether the magician's social cueing was directed toward the illusion, hidden behind a mask, or stationary. In a third experiment (Exp. 3), we replicated Kuhn and Land's initial results and attempted to further explain their "anti-illusion" social-cue effect. This study confirms that social cueing is not required in the VBI: Its presence did not increase the number of participants experiencing the illusion.


Asunto(s)
Anticipación Psicológica/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Ilusiones/fisiología , Ilusiones/psicología , Relaciones Interpersonales , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Movimientos Oculares/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto Joven
17.
Trends Cogn Sci ; 19(9): 524-33, 2015 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26255970

RESUMEN

Since its inception, the contextual cueing (CC) paradigm has generated considerable interest in various fields of cognitive sciences because it constitutes an elegant approach to understanding how statistical learning (SL) mechanisms can detect contextual regularities during a visual search. In this article we review and discuss five aspects of CC: (i) the implicit nature of learning, (ii) the mechanisms involved in CC, (iii) the mediating factors affecting CC, (iv) the generalization of CC phenomena, and (v) the dissociation between implicit and explicit CC phenomena. The findings suggest that implicit SL is an inherent component of ongoing processing which operates through clustering, associative, and reinforcement processes at various levels of sensory-motor processing, and might result from simple spike-timing-dependent plasticity.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/fisiología , Cognición/fisiología , Señales (Psicología) , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Atención , Mapeo Encefálico , Humanos , Estimulación Luminosa
18.
Br J Psychol ; 105(2): 162-72, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24754805

RESUMEN

Do sexual words have high attentional priority? How does the ability to ignore sexual distractors evolve with age? To answer these questions, two experiments using Rapid Serial Visual Presentation (RSVP) were conducted. Experiment 1 showed that both younger and older participants were better at identifying a target (the name of a colour) when it was preceded by 336 ms by a sexual word rather than by a musical word. Strikingly, the sexual-word advantage was more pronounced for older adults than for younger adults. Experiment 2 showed that introducing a variable delay between the distractor and the target eliminated the sexual-word advantage. This finding suggests that the sexual-word advantage found in Experiment 1 was due to learning to utilize the sexual word as a temporal cue with a fixed duration between the distractor and the target. Contrary to previous research [Arnell et al., 2007, Emotion, 7, 465), neither experiment showed that sexual words produce an attentional blink.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Parpadeo Atencional/fisiología , Estimulación Luminosa , Vocabulario , Adolescente , Adulto , Factores de Edad , Anciano , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
19.
PLoS One ; 9(2): e89276, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24586652

RESUMEN

Does verbalizing a previously-seen complex visual stimulus influence its subsequent recollection? We investigated this question by examining the mediating role played by expertise level in fencing on the effects of verbalizing upon visual memory. Participants with three distinct levels of expertise in fencing (novices, intermediates, experts) performed seven trials. In each trial, they first watched four times a short video that displayed fencing movements. Then, half of them verbalized the previously-seen visual stimulus (i.e., the verbalization group), the other half carried out a hidden-word task (i.e., the non-verbalization group). Finally, all the participants were asked to recognize the previously-seen fencing movements amongst novel fencing movements. Overall, verbalizing improved recognition for novices, altered recognition for intermediates, and had no effect for experts. These findings replicated the classical verbal-overshadowing effect, while extending it to a more conceptual material. They also point out to some potential benefits and costs of verbalizing on visual memory, depending on the level of expertise.


Asunto(s)
Aptitud/fisiología , Memoria/fisiología , Movimiento/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Habla/fisiología , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Lenguaje , Masculino , Adulto Joven
20.
Mem Cognit ; 42(2): 225-36, 2014 Feb.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24052189

RESUMEN

Substantial evidence has highlighted the ability of observers to incidentally extract statistical contingencies present in visual environments. This study examined whether the knowledge extracted regarding statistical contingencies is unconscious initially, even when it becomes fully accessible to conscious awareness after extensive training. Using a "typical" contextual cuing procedure adapted to real-world scenes, we first observed that, after extensive training in searching for a target within repeated scenes, knowledge about regularities was associated with conscious awareness (Experiment 1). However, both subjective and objective measures of consciousness revealed that in the early phase of training, learning of regular structures first takes place at an unconscious level (Experiments 2 and 3). These results are discussed in the light of the causal relationships between learning and consciousness.


Asunto(s)
Concienciación/fisiología , Estado de Conciencia/fisiología , Aprendizaje/fisiología , Inconsciente en Psicología , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Humanos , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA