Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 7 de 7
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
Exp Psychol ; 61(6): 417-38, 2014.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24962121

RESUMEN

We developed a novel four-dimensional spatial task called Shapebuilder and used it to predict performance on a wide variety of cognitive tasks. In six experiments, we illustrate that Shapebuilder: (1) Loads on a common factor with complex working memory (WM) span tasks and that it predicts performance on quantitative reasoning tasks and Ravens Progressive Matrices (Experiment 1), (2) Correlates well with traditional complex WM span tasks (Experiment 2), predicts performance on the conditional go/no go task (Experiment 3) and N-back (Experiment 4), and showed weak or nonsignificant correlations with the Attention Networks Task (Experiment 5), and task switching (Experiment 6). Shapebuilder shows that it exhibits minimal skew and kurtosis, and shows good reliability. We argue that Shapebuilder has many advantages over existing measures of WM, including the fact that it is largely language independent, is not prone to ceiling effects, and take less than 6 min to complete on average.


Asunto(s)
Cognición , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Análisis y Desempeño de Tareas , Adolescente , Adulto , Atención , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Test de Stroop , Adulto Joven
2.
Acta Psychol (Amst) ; 148: 19-24, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24486803

RESUMEN

We investigated the effects of 10h of practice on variations of the N-Back task to investigate the processes underlying possible expansion of the focus of attention within working memory. Using subtractive logic, we showed that random access (i.e., Sternberg-like search) yielded a modest effect (a 50% increase in speed) whereas the processes of forward access (i.e., retrieval in order, as in a standard N-Back task) and updating (i.e., changing the contents of working memory) were executed about 5 times faster after extended practice. We additionally found that extended practice increased working memory capacity as measured by the size of the focus of attention for the forward-access task, but not for variations where probing was in random order. This suggests that working memory capacity may depend on the type of search process engaged, and that certain working-memory-related cognitive processes are more amenable to practice than others.


Asunto(s)
Atención/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Práctica Psicológica , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas
3.
Conscious Cogn ; 22(1): 231-6, 2013 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23357240

RESUMEN

Alcohol use has long been assumed to alter cognition via attentional processes. To better understand the cognitive consequences of intoxication, the present study tested the effects of moderate intoxication (average BAC between .071 and .082) on attentional processing using complex working memory capacity (WMC) span tasks and a change blindness task. Intoxicated and sober participants were matched on baseline WMC performance, and intoxication significantly decreased performance on the complex span tasks. Surprisingly, intoxication improved performance on the change blindness task. The results are interpreted as evidence that intoxication decreases attentional control, causing either a shift towards more passive processing and/or a more diffuse attentional state. This may result in decreased performance on tasks where attentional control or focus are required, but may actually facilitate performance in some contexts.


Asunto(s)
Intoxicación Alcohólica/psicología , Atención , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino
4.
Conscious Cogn ; 21(1): 487-93, 2012 Mar.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22285424

RESUMEN

That alcohol provides a benefit to creative processes has long been assumed by popular culture, but to date has not been tested. The current experiment tested the effects of moderate alcohol intoxication on a common creative problem solving task, the Remote Associates Test (RAT). Individuals were brought to a blood alcohol content of approximately .075, and, after reaching peak intoxication, completed a battery of RAT items. Intoxicated individuals solved more RAT items, in less time, and were more likely to perceive their solutions as the result of a sudden insight. Results are interpreted from an attentional control perspective.


Asunto(s)
Intoxicación Alcohólica/psicología , Atención , Creatividad , Función Ejecutiva , Solución de Problemas , Adulto , Humanos , Masculino , Estados Unidos , Pruebas de Asociación de Palabras
5.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 37(1): 256-63, 2011 Jan.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21244117

RESUMEN

The correlation between individual differences in working memory capacity and performance on the Raven's Advanced Progressive Matrices (RAPM) is well documented yet poorly understood. The present work proposes a new explanation: that the need to use a new combination of rules on RAPM problems drives the relation between performance and working memory capacity scores. Evidence for this account is supported by an item-based analysis of performance during standard administration of the RAPM and an experiment that manipulates the need to use new rule combinations across 2 subsets of RAPM items. (PsycINFO Database Record (c) 2010 APA, all rights reserved).


Asunto(s)
Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Solución de Problemas/fisiología , Estadística como Asunto , Adaptación Psicológica , Femenino , Humanos , Pruebas de Inteligencia , Masculino , Pruebas Neuropsicológicas , Estimulación Luminosa , Valor Predictivo de las Pruebas , Análisis de Regresión , Estudiantes , Universidades
6.
Psychon Bull Rev ; 14(4): 699-703, 2007 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17972736

RESUMEN

The controlled attention theory of working memory suggests that individuals with greater working memory capacity (WMC) are better able to control or focus their attention than individuals with lesser WMC. This relationship has been observed in a number of selective attention paradigms including a dichotic listening task (Conway, Cowan, & Bunting, 2001) in which participants were required to shadow words presented to one ear and ignore words presented to the other ear. Conway et al. found that when the participant's name was presented to the ignored ear, 65% of participants with low WMC reported hearing their name, compared to only 20% of participants with high WMC, suggesting greater selective attention on the part of high WMC participants. In the present study, individual differences in divided attention were examined in a dichotic listening task, in which participants shadowed one message and listened for their own name in the other message. Here we find that 66.7% of high WMC and 34.5% of low WMC participants detected their name. These results suggest that as WMC capacity increases, so does the ability to control the focus of attention, with high WMC participants being able to flexibly "zoom in" or "zoom out" depending on task demands.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Pruebas de Audición Dicótica , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Adulto , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Tamizaje Masivo
7.
J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn ; 33(3): 615-622, 2007 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17470009

RESUMEN

The N-back task requires participants to decide whether each stimulus in a sequence matches the one that appeared n items ago. Although N-back has become a standard "executive" working memory (WM) measure in cognitive neuroscience, it has been subjected to few behavioral tests of construct validity. A combined experimental- correlational study tested the attention-control demands of verbal 2- and 3-back tasks by presenting n = 1 "lure" foils. Lures elicited more false alarms than control foils in both 2- and 3-back tasks, and lures caused more misses to targets that immediately followed them compared with control targets, but only in 3-back tasks. N-back thus challenges control over familiarity-based responding. Participants also completed a verbal WM span task (operation span task) and a marker test of general fluid intelligence (Gf; Ravens Advanced Progressive Matrices Test; J. C. Raven, J. E. Raven, & J. H. Court, 1998). N-back and WM span correlated weakly, suggesting they do not reflect primarily a single construct; moreover, both accounted for independent variance in Gf. N-back has face validity as a WM task, but it does not demonstrate convergent validity with at least 1 established WM measure.


Asunto(s)
Atención , Memoria a Corto Plazo , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Aprendizaje Seriado , Humanos , Individualidad , Inteligencia , Solución de Problemas , Desempeño Psicomotor , Tiempo de Reacción , Semántica , Aprendizaje Verbal
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA