Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 11 de 11
Filtrar
Más filtros











Base de datos
Intervalo de año de publicación
1.
J Vis ; 23(7): 8, 2023 07 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37432844

RESUMEN

When we look at an object, we simultaneously see how glossy or matte it is, how light or dark, and what color. Yet, at each point on the object's surface, both diffuse and specular reflections are mixed in different proportions, resulting in substantial spatial chromatic and luminance variations. To further complicate matters, this pattern changes radically when the object is viewed under different lighting conditions. The purpose of this study was to simultaneously measure our ability to judge color and gloss using an image set capturing diverse object and illuminant properties. Participants adjusted the hue, lightness, chroma, and specular reflectance of a reference object so that it appeared to be made of the same material as a test object. Critically, the two objects were presented under different lighting environments. We found that hue matches were highly accurate, except for under a chromatically atypical illuminant. Chroma and lightness constancy were generally poor, but these failures correlated well with simple image statistics. Gloss constancy was particularly poor, and these failures were only partially explained by reflection contrast. Importantly, across all measures, participants were highly consistent with one another in their deviations from constancy. Although color and gloss constancy hold well in simple conditions, the variety of lighting and shape in the real world presents significant challenges to our visual system's ability to judge intrinsic material properties.


Asunto(s)
Iluminación , Humanos
2.
iScience ; 25(3): 103970, 2022 Mar 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35281732

RESUMEN

Many natural materials have complex, multi-scale structures. Consequently, the inferred identity of a surface can vary with the assumed spatial scale of the scene: a plowed field seen from afar can resemble corduroy seen up close. We investigated this 'material-scale ambiguity' using 87 photographs of diverse materials (e.g., water, sand, stone, metal, and wood). Across two experiments, separate groups of participants (N = 72 adults) provided judgements of the material category depicted in each image, either with or without manipulations of apparent distance (by verbal instructions, or adding objects of familiar size). Our results demonstrate that these manipulations can cause identical images to be assigned to completely different material categories, depending on the assumed scale. Under challenging conditions, therefore, the categorization of materials is susceptible to simple manipulations of apparent distance, revealing a striking example of top-down effects in the interpretation of image features.

3.
J Opt Soc Am A Opt Image Sci Vis ; 38(2): 203-210, 2021 Feb 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33690530

RESUMEN

While much attention has been given to understanding biases in gloss perception (e.g., changes in perceived reflectance as a function of lighting, shape, viewpoint, and other factors), here we investigated sensitivity to changes in surface reflectance. We tested how visual sensitivity to differences in specular reflectance varies as a function of the magnitude of specular reflectance. Stimuli consisted of renderings of glossy objects under natural illumination. Using maximum likelihood difference scaling (MLDS), we created a perceptual scaling of the specular reflectance parameter of the Ward reflectance model. Then, using the method of constant stimuli and a standard 2AFC procedure, we obtained psychometric functions for gloss discrimination across a range of reflectance values derived from the perceptual scale. Both methods demonstrate that discriminability is significantly diminished at high levels of specular reflectance, thus indicating that gloss sensitivity depends on the magnitude of change in the image produced by different reflectance values. Taken together, these experiments also suggest that internal sensory noise remains constant for suprathreshold and near-threshold intervals of specular reflectance, which supports the use of MLDS as a highly efficient method for evaluating gloss sensitivity.

4.
Sci Rep ; 6: 25473, 2016 05 03.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27137417

RESUMEN

Our tactual perceptual experiences occur when we interact, actively and passively, with environmental objects and surfaces. Previous research has demonstrated that active manual exploration often enhances the tactual perception of object shape. Nevertheless, the factors that contribute to this enhancement are not well understood. The present study evaluated the ability of 28 younger (mean age was 23.1 years) and older adults (mean age was 71.4 years) to discriminate curved surfaces by actively feeling objects with a single index finger and by passively feeling objects that moved relative to a restrained finger. While dynamic cutaneous stimulation was therefore present in both conditions, active exploratory movements only occurred in one. The results indicated that there was a significant and large effect of age, such that the older participants' thresholds were 43.8 percent higher than those of the younger participants. Despite the overall adverse effect of age, the pattern of results across the active and passive touch conditions was identical. For both age groups, the curvature discrimination thresholds obtained for passive touch were significantly lower than those that occurred during active touch. Curvature discrimination performance was therefore best in the current study when dynamic cutaneous stimulation occurred in the absence of active movement.


Asunto(s)
Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Fenómenos Fisiológicos de la Piel , Tacto/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Envejecimiento/patología , Envejecimiento/fisiología , Femenino , Dedos/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Movimiento
5.
PLoS One ; 11(2): e0149058, 2016.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26863531

RESUMEN

It is well known that motion facilitates the visual perception of solid object shape, particularly when surface texture or other identifiable features (e.g., corners) are present. Conventional models of structure-from-motion require the presence of texture or identifiable object features in order to recover 3-D structure. Is the facilitation in 3-D shape perception similar in magnitude when surface texture is absent? On any given trial in the current experiments, participants were presented with a single randomly-selected solid object (bell pepper or randomly-shaped "glaven") for 12 seconds and were required to indicate which of 12 (for bell peppers) or 8 (for glavens) simultaneously visible objects possessed the same shape. The initial single object's shape was defined either by boundary contours alone (i.e., presented as a silhouette), specular highlights alone, specular highlights combined with boundary contours, or texture. In addition, there was a haptic condition: in this condition, the participants haptically explored with both hands (but could not see) the initial single object for 12 seconds; they then performed the same shape-matching task used in the visual conditions. For both the visual and haptic conditions, motion (rotation in depth or active object manipulation) was present in half of the trials and was not present for the remaining trials. The effect of motion was quantitatively similar for all of the visual and haptic conditions-e.g., the participants' performance in Experiment 1 was 93.5 percent higher in the motion or active haptic manipulation conditions (when compared to the static conditions). The current results demonstrate that deforming specular highlights or boundary contours facilitate 3-D shape perception as much as the motion of objects that possess texture. The current results also indicate that the improvement with motion that occurs for haptics is similar in magnitude to that which occurs for vision.


Asunto(s)
Percepción de Forma , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Percepción Visual , Adulto , Algoritmos , Gráficos por Computador , Computadores , Percepción de Profundidad , Discriminación en Psicología , Humanos , Cinética , Movimiento (Física) , Tiempo de Reacción , Reconocimiento en Psicología , Tacto , Adulto Joven
6.
Vision Res ; 115(Pt A): 113-8, 2015 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26342965

RESUMEN

The ability of 114 younger and older adults to recognize naturally-shaped objects was evaluated in three experiments. The participants viewed or haptically explored six randomly-chosen bell peppers (Capsicum annuum) in a study session and were later required to judge whether each of twelve bell peppers was "old" (previously presented during the study session) or "new" (not presented during the study session). When recognition memory was tested immediately after study, the younger adults' (Experiment 1) performance for vision and haptics was identical when the individual study objects were presented once. Vision became superior to haptics, however, when the individual study objects were presented multiple times. When 10- and 20-min delays (Experiment 2) were inserted in between study and test sessions, no significant differences occurred between vision and haptics: recognition performance in both modalities was comparable. When the recognition performance of older adults was evaluated (Experiment 3), a negative effect of age was found for visual shape recognition (younger adults' overall recognition performance was 60% higher). There was no age effect, however, for haptic shape recognition. The results of the present experiments indicate that the visual recognition of natural object shape is different from haptic recognition in multiple ways: visual shape recognition can be superior to that of haptics and is affected by aging, while haptic shape recognition is less accurate and unaffected by aging.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción Visual/fisiología , Adulto , Análisis de Varianza , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Verduras , Adulto Joven
7.
Vision Res ; 98: 89-98, 2014 May.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24705076

RESUMEN

Younger (20-25 years of age) and older (61-79 years) adults were evaluated for their ability to visually discriminate length. Almost all experiments that have utilized the method of single stimuli to date have required participants to judge test stimuli relative to a single implicit standard (for a rare exception, see Morgan, On the scaling of size judgements by orientational cues, Vision Research, 1992, 32, 1433-1445). In the current experiments, we not only asked participants to judge lengths relative to a single implicit standard, but they also compared test stimuli to two different implicit standards within the same blocks of trials. We analyzed our participants' judgments to evaluate whether significant sequential dependencies occurred. We found that while individual younger and older adults possessed similar length difference thresholds and exhibited similar overall biases, the judgments of older adults within individual blocks of trials were more strongly biased (than younger adults) by preceding responses (i.e., their judgments on any given trial were more strongly affected by responses to previously viewed stimuli). In addition, the judgments of both younger and older adults were more strongly biased by preceding responses in the blocks of trials with multiple implicit standards. Overall, our results are consistent with the operation of the tracking mechanism described by Criterion-setting theory (Lages and Treisman, Spatial frequency discrimination: Visual long-term memory or criterion setting? Vision Research, 1998, 38, 557-572).


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Memoria a Corto Plazo/fisiología , Percepción del Tamaño/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Psicometría , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Adulto Joven
8.
Vision Res ; 93: 54-61, 2013 Dec 18.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24157595

RESUMEN

Two experiments evaluated the ability of 50 older, middle-aged, and younger adults to discriminate the 3-dimensional (3-D) shape of curved surfaces defined by optical motion. In Experiment 1, temporal correspondence was disrupted by limiting the lifetimes of the moving surface points. In order to discriminate 3-D surface shape reliably, the younger and middle-aged adults needed a surface point lifetime of approximately 4 views (in the apparent motion sequences). In contrast, the older adults needed a much longer surface point lifetime of approximately 9 views in order to reliably perform the same task. In Experiment 2, the negative effect of age upon 3-D shape discrimination from motion was replicated. In this experiment, however, the participants' abilities to discriminate grating orientation and speed were also assessed. Edden et al. (2009) have recently demonstrated that behavioral grating orientation discrimination correlates with GABA (gamma aminobutyric acid) concentration in human visual cortex. Our results demonstrate that the negative effect of age upon 3-D shape perception from motion is not caused by impairments in the ability to perceive motion per se, but does correlate significantly with grating orientation discrimination. This result suggests that the age-related decline in 3-D shape discrimination from motion is related to decline in GABA concentration in visual cortex.


Asunto(s)
Factores de Edad , Percepción de Forma/fisiología , Percepción de Movimiento/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis de Varianza , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Estimulación Luminosa/métodos , Adulto Joven
9.
PLoS One ; 8(7): e68577, 2013.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-23844224

RESUMEN

Two experiments evaluated the ability of 30 older and younger adults to discriminate the curvature of simple object surfaces from static and dynamic touch. The ages of the older adults ranged from 66 to 85 years, while those of the younger adults ranged from 20 to 29 years. For each participant in both experiments, the minimum curvature magnitude needed to reliably discriminate between convex and concave surfaces was determined. In Experiment 1, participants used static touch to make their judgments of curvature, while dynamic touch was used in Experiment 2. When static touch was used to discriminate curvature, a large effect of age occurred (the thresholds were 0.67 & 1.11/m for the younger and older participants, respectively). However, when participants used dynamic touch, there was no significant difference between the ability of younger and older participants to discriminate curvature (the thresholds were 0.58 & 0.59/m for the younger and older participants, respectively). The results of the current study demonstrate that while older adults can accurately discriminate surface curvature from dynamic touch, they possess significant impairments for static touch.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/fisiología , Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Juicio , Tacto/fisiología , Adulto , Anciano , Anciano de 80 o más Años , Análisis de Varianza , Dedos/fisiología , Humanos , Persona de Mediana Edad , Desempeño Psicomotor/fisiología , Umbral Sensorial/fisiología , Estereognosis/fisiología , Propiedades de Superficie , Adulto Joven
10.
Exp Brain Res ; 222(3): 321-32, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22918607

RESUMEN

A set of three experiments evaluated 96 participants' ability to visually and haptically discriminate solid object shape. In the past, some researchers have found haptic shape discrimination to be substantially inferior to visual shape discrimination, while other researchers have found haptics and vision to be essentially equivalent. A primary goal of the present study was to understand these discrepant past findings and to determine the true capabilities of the haptic system. All experiments used the same task (same vs. different shape discrimination) and stimulus objects (James Gibson's "feelies" and a set of naturally shaped objects--bell peppers). However, the methodology varied across experiments. Experiment 1 used random 3-dimensional (3-D) orientations of the stimulus objects, and the conditions were full-cue (active manipulation of objects and rotation of the visual objects in depth). Experiment 2 restricted the 3-D orientations of the stimulus objects and limited the haptic and visual information available to the participants. Experiment 3 compared restricted and full-cue conditions using random 3-D orientations. We replicated both previous findings in the current study. When we restricted visual and haptic information (and placed the stimulus objects in the same orientation on every trial), the participants' visual performance was superior to that obtained for haptics (replicating the earlier findings of Davidson et al. in Percept Psychophys 15(3):539-543, 1974). When the circumstances resembled those of ordinary life (e.g., participants able to actively manipulate objects and see them from a variety of perspectives), we found no significant difference between visual and haptic solid shape discrimination.


Asunto(s)
Discriminación en Psicología/fisiología , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos/fisiología , Reconocimiento en Psicología/fisiología , Percepción del Tacto/fisiología , Tacto/fisiología , Adolescente , Adulto , Señales (Psicología) , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Orientación , Estimulación Luminosa , Tiempo de Reacción , Adulto Joven
11.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 74(7): 1512-21, 2012 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22766899

RESUMEN

Two experiments evaluated the ability of younger and older adults to visually discriminate 3-D shape as a function of surface coherence. The coherence was manipulated by embedding the 3-D surfaces in volumetric noise (e.g., for a 55 % coherent surface, 55 % of the stimulus points fell on a 3-D surface, while 45 % of the points occupied random locations within the same volume of space). The 3-D surfaces were defined by static binocular disparity, dynamic binocular disparity, and motion. The results of both experiments demonstrated significant effects of age: Older adults required more coherence (tolerated volumetric noise less) for reliable shape discrimination than did younger adults. Motion-defined and static-binocular-disparity-defined surfaces resulted in similar coherence thresholds. However, performance for dynamic-binocular-disparity-defined surfaces was superior (i.e., the observers' surface coherence thresholds were lowest for these stimuli). The results of both experiments showed that younger and older adults possess considerable tolerance to the disrupting effects of volumetric noise; the observers could reliably discriminate 3-D surface shape even when 45 % of the stimulus points (or more) constituted noise.


Asunto(s)
Envejecimiento/psicología , Percepción de Profundidad , Aprendizaje Discriminativo , Percepción de Movimiento , Reconocimiento Visual de Modelos , Disparidad Visual , Adolescente , Anciano , Atención , Discriminación en Psicología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Orientación , Valores de Referencia , Propiedades de Superficie , Adulto Joven
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA