Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 72
Filtrar
1.
J Biol Rhythms ; 37(4): 403-416, 2022 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35686534

RESUMEN

Light-induced improvements in alertness are more prominent during nighttime than during the day, suggesting that alerting effects of light may depend on internal clock time or wake duration. Relative contributions of both factors can be quantified using a forced desynchrony (FD) designs. FD designs have only been conducted under dim light conditions (<10 lux) since light above this amount can induce non-uniform phase progression of the circadian pacemaker (also called relative coordination). This complicates the mathematical separation of circadian clock phase from homeostatic sleep pressure effects. Here we investigate alerting effects of light in a novel 4 × 18 h FD protocol (5 h sleep, 13 h wake) under dim (6 lux) and bright light (1300 lux) conditions. Hourly saliva samples (melatonin and cortisol assessment) and 2-hourly test sessions were used to assess effects of bright light on subjective and objective alertness (electroencephalography and performance). Results reveal (1) stable free-running cortisol rhythms with uniform phase progression under both light conditions, suggesting that FD designs can be conducted under bright light conditions (1300 lux), (2) subjective alerting effects of light depend on elapsed time awake but not circadian clock phase, while (3) light consistently improves objective alertness independent of time awake or circadian clock phase. Reconstructing the daily time course by combining circadian clock phase and wake duration effects indicates that performance is improved during daytime, while subjective alertness remains unchanged. This suggests that high-intensity indoor lighting during the regular day might be beneficial for mental performance, even though this may not be perceived as such.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Melatonina , Humanos , Hidrocortisona , Masculino , Sueño , Vigilia
2.
J Biol Rhythms ; 37(4): 417-428, 2022 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35723003

RESUMEN

Human thermoregulation is strictly regulated by the preoptic area of the hypothalamus, which is directly influenced by the suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN). The main input pathway of the SCN is light. Here, thermoregulatory effects of light were assessed in humans in a forced desynchrony (FD) design. The FD experiment was performed in dim light (DL, 6 lux) and bright white light (BL, 1300 lux) in 8 men in a semi-randomized within-subject design. A 4 × 18 h FD protocol (5 h sleep, 13 h wake) was applied, with continuous core body temperature (CBT) and skin temperature measurements at the forehead, clavicles, navel, palms, foot soles and toes. Skin temperature parameters indicated sleep-wake modulations as well as internal clock variations. All distal skin temperature parameters increased during sleep, when CBT decreased. Light significantly affected temperature levels during the wake phase, with decreased temperature measured at the forehead and toes and increased navel and clavicular skin temperatures. These effects persisted when the lights were turned off for sleep. Circadian amplitude of CBT and all skin temperature parameters decreased significantly during BL exposure. Circadian proximal skin temperatures cycled in phase with CBT, while distal skin temperatures cycled in anti-phase, confirming the idea that distal skin regions reflect heat dissipation and proximal regions approximate CBT. In general, we find that increased light intensity exposure may have decreased heat loss in humans, especially at times when the circadian system promotes sleep.


Asunto(s)
Melatonina , Temperatura Cutánea , Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Regulación de la Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Humanos , Masculino , Melatonina/metabolismo , Sueño/fisiología
3.
J Biol Rhythms ; 37(4): 429-441, 2022 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-35730553

RESUMEN

Under real-life conditions, increased light exposure during wakefulness seems associated with improved sleep quality, quantified as reduced time awake during bed time, increased time spent in non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep, or increased power of the electroencephalogram delta band (0.5-4 Hz). The causality of these important relationships and their dependency on circadian phase and/or time awake has not been studied in depth. To disentangle possible circadian and homeostatic interactions, we employed a forced desynchrony protocol under dim light (6 lux) and under bright light (1300 lux) during wakefulness. Our protocol consisted of a fast cycling sleep-wake schedule (13 h wakefulness-5 h sleep; 4 cycles), followed by 3 h recovery sleep in a within-subject cross-over design. Individuals (8 men) were equipped with 10 polysomnography electrodes. Subjective sleep quality was measured immediately after wakening with a questionnaire. Results indicated that circadian variation in delta power was only detected under dim light. Circadian variation in time in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep and wakefulness were uninfluenced by light. Prior light exposure increased accumulation of delta power and time in NREM sleep, while it decreased wakefulness, especially during the circadian wake phase (biological day). Subjective sleep quality scores showed that participants rated their sleep quality better after bright light exposure while sleeping when the circadian system promoted wakefulness. These results suggest that high environmental light intensity either increases sleep pressure buildup during wakefulness or prevents the occurrence of micro-sleep, leading to improved quality of subsequent sleep.


Asunto(s)
Calidad del Sueño , Vigilia , Ritmo Circadiano , Humanos , Luz , Masculino , Sueño , Sueño REM
4.
Sci Rep ; 10(1): 16088, 2020 10 08.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-33033271

RESUMEN

The circadian system affects physiological, psychological, and molecular mechanisms in the body, resulting in varying physical performance over the day. The timing and relative size of these effects are important for optimizing sport performance. In this study, Olympic swim times (from 2004 to 2016) were used to determine time-of-day and circadian effects under maximal motivational conditions. Data of athletes who made it to the finals (N = 144, 72 female) were included and normalized on individual levels based on the average swim times over race types (heat, semifinal, and final) per individual for each stroke, distance and Olympic venue. Normalized swim times were analyzed with a linear mixed model and a sine fitted model. Swim performance was better during finals as compared to semi-finals and heats. Performance was strongly affected by time-of-day, showing fastest swim times in the late afternoon around 17:12 h, indicating 0.32% improved performance relative to 08:00 h. This study reveals clear effects of time-of-day on physical performance in Olympic athletes. The time-of-day effect is large, and exceeds the time difference between gold and silver medal in 40%, silver and bronze medal in 64%, and bronze or no medal in 61% of the finals.


Asunto(s)
Atletas/psicología , Rendimiento Atlético/psicología , Rendimiento Atlético/estadística & datos numéricos , Ritmo Circadiano , Conducta Competitiva/fisiología , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino
5.
Sci Rep ; 8(1): 15214, 2018 10 12.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30315193

RESUMEN

The mammalian circadian system encodes both absolute levels of light intensity and color to phase-lock (entrain) its rhythm to the 24-h solar cycle. The evolutionary benefits of circadian color-coding over intensity-coding per se are yet far from understood. A detailed characterization of sunlight is crucial in understanding how and why circadian photoreception integrates color and intensity information. To this end, we continuously measured 100 days of sunlight spectra over the course of a year. Our analyses suggest that circadian color-coding may have evolved to cope with cloud-induced variation in light intensity. We proceed to show how an integration of intensity and spectral composition reduces day-to-day variability in the synchronizing signal (Zeitgeber). As a consequence, entrained phase angle of the circadian clock will be more stable, which will be beneficial for the organism. The presented characterization of sunlight dynamics may become important in designing lighting solutions aimed at minimizing the detrimental effects of light at night in modern societies.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Luz Solar , Color , Humanos , Modelos Teóricos , Factores de Tiempo
6.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27449361

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Mood disorders constitute a high burden for both patients and society. Notwithstanding the large arsenal of available treatment options, a considerable group of patients does not remit on current antidepressant treatment. There is an urgent need to develop alternative treatment strategies. Recently, low-strength transcranial pulsed electromagnetic field (tPEMF) stimulation has been purported as a promising strategy for such treatment-resistant depression (TRD). The mode of action of this new technique is however largely unknown. METHODS: We searched PubMed for literature reports on the effects of tPEMF and for information regarding its working mechanism and biological substrate. RESULTS: Most studies more or less connect with the major hypotheses of depression and concern the effects of tPEMF on brain metabolism, neuronal connectivity, brain plasticity, and the immune system. Relatively few studies paid attention to the possible chronobiologic effects of electromagnetic fields. LIMITATIONS: We reviewed the literature of a new and still developing field. Some of the reports involved translational studies, which inevitably limits the reach of the conclusions. CONCLUSION: Weak magnetic fields influence divergent neurobiological processes. The antidepressant effect of tPEMF may be specifically attributable to its effects on local brain activity and connectivity.


Asunto(s)
Encéfalo/metabolismo , Depresión/terapia , Estimulación Magnética Transcraneal/métodos , Humanos , PubMed/estadística & datos numéricos
7.
Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci ; 366(1574): 2141-54, 2011 Jul 27.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-21690131

RESUMEN

Virtually all species have developed cellular oscillations and mechanisms that synchronize these cellular oscillations to environmental cycles. Such environmental cycles in biotic (e.g. food availability and predation risk) or abiotic (e.g. temperature and light) factors may occur on a daily, annual or tidal time scale. Internal timing mechanisms may facilitate behavioural or physiological adaptation to such changes in environmental conditions. These timing mechanisms commonly involve an internal molecular oscillator (a 'clock') that is synchronized ('entrained') to the environmental cycle by receptor mechanisms responding to relevant environmental signals ('Zeitgeber', i.e. German for time-giver). To understand the evolution of such timing mechanisms, we have to understand the mechanisms leading to selective advantage. Although major advances have been made in our understanding of the physiological and molecular mechanisms driving internal cycles (proximate questions), studies identifying mechanisms of natural selection on clock systems (ultimate questions) are rather limited. Here, we discuss the selective advantage of a circadian system and how its adaptation to day length variation may have a functional role in optimizing seasonal timing. We discuss various cases where selective advantages of circadian timing mechanisms have been shown and cases where temporarily loss of circadian timing may cause selective advantage. We suggest an explanation for why a circadian timing system has emerged in primitive life forms like cyanobacteria and we evaluate a possible molecular mechanism that enabled these bacteria to adapt to seasonal variation in day length. We further discuss how the role of the circadian system in photoperiodic time measurement may explain differential selection pressures on circadian period when species are exposed to changing climatic conditions (e.g. global warming) or when they expand their geographical range to different latitudes or altitudes.


Asunto(s)
Adaptación Fisiológica , Relojes Biológicos/genética , Evolución Biológica , Fotoperiodo , Animales , Arabidopsis , Relojes Biológicos/fisiología , Encéfalo/fisiología , Péptidos y Proteínas de Señalización del Ritmo Circadiano/genética , Péptidos y Proteínas de Señalización del Ritmo Circadiano/metabolismo , Regulación de la Expresión Génica/fisiología , Luz
8.
J Biol Rhythms ; 23(5): 425-34, 2008 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-18838608

RESUMEN

Phase shifting of circadian systems by light has been attributed both to parametric effects on angular velocity elicited by a tonic response to the luminance level and to nonparametric instantaneous shifts induced by a phasic response to the dark-light (D>L) and light-dark (L>D) transitions. Claims of nonparametric responses are partly based on "step-PRCs," that is, phase response curves derived from such transitions. Step-PRCs in nocturnal mammals show mostly delays after lights-on and advances after lights-off, and therefore appear incompatible with phase delays generated by light around dusk and advances by light around dawn. We have pursued this paradox with 2 experimental protocols in mice. We first use the classic step-PRC protocol on wheel running activity, using the center of gravity as a phase marker to minimize the masking effects of light. The experiment was done for 3 different light intensities (1, 10, and 100 lux). D>L transitions evoke mostly delays and L>D transitions show no clear tendency to either delay or advance. Overall there is little or no circadian modulation. A 2nd protocol aimed to avoid the problem of masking by assessing phase before and after the light stimuli, both in DD. Light stimuli consisted of either a slow light intensity increase over 48 h followed by abruptly switching off the light, or an abrupt switch on followed by a slow decrease toward total darkness during 48 h. If the abrupt transitions were responsible for phase shifting, we expected large differences between the 2 stimuli. Both light stimuli yielded similar PRCs characterized by delays only with circadian modulation. The results can be adequately explained by a model in which all PRCs evoked by steps result in fact from tonic responses to the light following a step-up or preceding a step-down. In this model only the response reduction of tonic velocity change after the 1st hour is taken into account. The data obtained in both experiments are thus compatible with tonic velocity responses. Contrary to standard interpretation of step-PRCs, nonparametric responses to the transitions are unlikely since they would predict delays in response to lights-off, advances in response to lights-on, while the opposite was found. Although such responses cannot be fully excluded, parsimony does not require invocation of a role for transitions, since all the data can readily be explained by tonic velocity (parametric) effects, which must exist because of the dependence of tau on light intensity.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Luz , Fotoperiodo , Animales , Oscuridad , Locomoción , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Modelos Biológicos , Estimulación Luminosa , Análisis de Regresión , Factores de Tiempo
9.
J Biol Rhythms ; 22(5): 432-44, 2007 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-17876064

RESUMEN

Entrainment may involve responses to dawn, to dusk, and to the light in between these transitions. Previous studies showed that the circadian system responds to only 2 light pulses, one at the beginning and one at the end of the day, in a similar way as to a full photoperiod, as long as the photoperiod is less than approximately 1/2 tau. The authors used a double 1-h light pulse protocol with different intervals of darkness in between (1, 2, 4, 7, 10, and 16 h) to study the phase responses of mice. The phase response curves obtained were compared to full light pulse PRCs of corresponding durations. Up to 6 hours, phase responses induced by double light pulses are virtually the same as by a corresponding full light pulse. The authors made a simple phase-only model to estimate the response reduction due to light exposure and response restoration due to dark exposure of the system. In this model, they assumed a 100% contribution of the first 1-h light pulse and fitted the reduction factor for the second light pulse to yield the best fit to the observations. The results suggest that after 1 h of light followed by less than 4 h of darkness, there is a considerable reduction in response to the second light pulse. Full response restoration requires more than 10 h of darkness. To investigate the influence of the duration of light on the response saturation, the authors performed a second series of experiments where the duration of the 2 light pulses was varied from 4 to 60 min each with a fixed duration of the stimulus (4 h). The response to 2 light pulses saturates when they are between 30 and 60 min long. In conclusion, double pulses replace single full light pulses of a corresponding duration of up to 6 h due to a response reduction during light, combined with response restoration during darkness. By the combined response reduction and response restoration, mice can maintain stable entrainment to the external LD cycle without being continuously exposed to it.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Luz , Animales , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Factores de Tiempo
10.
J Biol Rhythms ; 21(5): 362-72, 2006 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-16998156

RESUMEN

To understand entrainment of circadian systems to different photoperiods in nature, it is important to know the effects of single light pulses of different durations on the free-running system. The authors studied the phase and period responses of laboratory mice (C57BL6J//OlaHsd) to single light pulses of 7 different durations (1, 3, 4, 6, 9, 12, and 18 h) given once per 11 days in otherwise constant darkness. Light-pulse duration affected both amplitude and shape of the phase response curve. Nine-hour light pulses yielded the maximal amplitude PRC. As in other systems, the circadian period slightly lengthened following delays and shortened following advances. The authors aimed to understand how different parts of the light signal contribute to the eventual phase shift. When PRCs were plotted using the onset, midpoint, and end of the pulse as a phase reference, they corresponded best with each other when using the mid-pulse. Using a simple phase-only model, the authors explored the possibility that light affects oscillator velocity strongly in the 1st hour and at reduced strength in later hours of the pulse due to photoreceptor adaptation. They fitted models based on the 1-h PRC to the data for all light pulses. The best overall correspondence between PRCs was obtained when the effect of light during all hours after the first was reduced by a factor of 0.22 relative to the 1st hour. For the predicted PRCs, the light action centered on average at 38% of the light pulse. This is close to the reference phase yielding best correspondence at 36% of the pulses. The result is thus compatible with an initial major contribution of the onset of the light pulse followed by a reduced effect of light responsible for the differences between PRCs for different duration pulses. The authors suggest that the mid-pulse is a better phase reference than lights-on to plot and compare PRCs of different light-pulse durations.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Luz , Aclimatación , Animales , Análisis de Fourier , Masculino , Ratones , Ratones Endogámicos C57BL , Modelos Biológicos , Fotoperiodo , Tiempo , Factores de Tiempo
11.
Biol Psychiatry ; 50(9): 691-8, 2001 Nov 01.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11704076

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: It has been hypothesized that the circadian pacemaker is phase delayed in seasonal affective disorder, (SAD) winter type, and that the phase advance resulting from morning ocular light accounts for the efficacy of light therapy. Extraocular light has been reported to produce phase-shifts of the human circadian pacemaker. This allows a double-blind, placebo-controlled study of light therapy in SAD. METHODS: Twenty-nine SAD patients participated. Clinical state was measured on days 1, 8, and 15 of the protocol. From days 4 through 8, 15 patients (4 M, 11 F) received extraocular light by fiberoptic illumination, and 14 (4 M, 10 F) placebo (no light) in the popliteal fossae, from 8 AM to 11 AM. In the evenings of days 3 and 8, the salivary dim light melatonin onset (DLMO) was assessed. Patients completed daily self-ratings on mood, alertness, and sleep. RESULTS: Both conditions showed a progressive improvement of clinical state over time. Between conditions, no significant differences were observed in clinical scores, the self-ratings on mood and alertness, and in timing of the DLMO before and directly after treatment. CONCLUSIONS: The response to extraocular light therapy in SAD patients did not exceed its placebo effect. Extraocular light did not induce a phase shift of the circadian pacemaker.


Asunto(s)
Fototerapia/métodos , Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/terapia , Adulto , Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Método Doble Ciego , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Melatonina/análisis , Melatonina/metabolismo , Saliva/química , Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/metabolismo , Encuestas y Cuestionarios
12.
J Affect Disord ; 66(2-3): 215-23, 2001 Oct.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-11578675

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Annual patterns in suicide rates, peaking near the summer solstice, are well documented. It has been suggested that day length or total hours of sunshine has an impact on suicide rates. If these environmental factors are involved, we would expect changes in the daily pattern of suicide rates to occur over the year. To test this hypothesis, the 24-h patterns of suicide rate were investigated as a function of time of year. METHOD: Detailed information about the exact time of suicides in The Netherlands is only available for train suicides. Therefore, information concerning age, sex, time and place of occurrence of all verified train suicides over 15 years in The Netherlands (n=2830) was obtained from The Netherlands Railways archives. RESULTS: Daily patterns in train suicides show systematic variations of two kinds. First, independently of time of year, suicide rates at night drop to about 10% of their daytime values. Second, there are two daily peaks in the patterns which shift their timing over the year, with one peak occurring shortly after sunset, and the other one consistently occurring 9-10 h earlier. Both peaks shift with the 5.5-h shift in sunset time. LIMITATIONS: Train suicidal behaviour may not represent fatal suicidal behaviour in general. CONCLUSIONS: There are pronounced and systematic daily variations in train suicide rates in The Netherlands. One of these is related to clock time, the others are related to the light-dark cycle. The consistency of the patterns suggests a strong environmental influence on train suicidal behaviour. Research on 24-h patterns of suicide rates should control for time of year.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano , Vías Férreas/estadística & datos numéricos , Estaciones del Año , Suicidio/estadística & datos numéricos , Adulto , Estudios Transversales , Femenino , Humanos , Incidencia , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Países Bajos/epidemiología
14.
Biol Psychiatry ; 47(4): 355-8, 2000 Feb 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10686271

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: It has been suggested that certain abnormalities (e.g., in phase or amplitude) of the circadian pacemaker underlie seasonal affective disorder. METHODS: One male seasonal affective disorder patient (blind to the study design) participated in two 120-hour forced desynchrony experiments and was subjected to six 20-hour days, once during a depressive episode and once after recovery. Core body temperature was continuously measured. During wakefulness, the Adjective Mood Scale was completed at 2-hour intervals. RESULTS: Sleep-wake as well as pacemaker-related variations of mood were found, both when the subject was depressed and when he was euthymic. Compared with recovery, during the depressive episode the circadian temperature minimum and the circadian mood variation showed phase delays of approximately 1 and 2 hours, respectively. CONCLUSIONS: The data of this first seasonal affective disorder patient, participating in forced desynchrony experiments, may indicate a phase delay of the circadian pacemaker during a seasonal affective disorder episode.


Asunto(s)
Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Trastornos del Humor/diagnóstico , Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/diagnóstico , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Escalas de Valoración Psiquiátrica , Recurrencia , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Sueño/fisiología , Encuestas y Cuestionarios , Vigilia/fisiología
15.
J Sleep Res ; 8(3): 163-74, 1999 Sep.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10476002

RESUMEN

The purpose of the study was to induce in two different ways, a phase-angle difference between the circadian pacemaker and the imposed sleep-wake cycle in humans, we intended to: (i) shift the circadian pacemaker by exposure to bright light and keep the timing of the sleep-wake cycle fixed; and (ii) keep the timing of the circadian pacemaker fixed by a constant light-dark cycle and displace sleep. We monitored dim light melatonin onset (DLMO), core body temperature and sleep. DLMO was delayed significantly after 3 days of a 3-h delayed sleep-phase when compared with 3 days of sleep at a normal or 3-h advanced sleep-phase. The shifts in DLMO were not accompanied by shifts in body temperature, changes in waking-up time or by a change in the duration of the first rapid eye movement (REM) sleep episode. Three days of light exposure in the morning or evening resulted in shifts in DLMO of similar magnitude, but this was accompanied by shifts in the rhythm of body temperature, changes in waking-up time and in the duration of the first REM sleep episode. We conclude that the changes observed after light exposure reflect shifts in the circadian pacemaker. In contrast, we propose that the changes observed in DLMO after sleep displacement are not mediated by the circadian pacemaker. These results raise some doubts about the reliability of DLMO as a marker of circadian phase in cases of sleep disturbances. Finally, we initiate a search for changes in sleep that might be responsible for the unexpected effects on DLMO.


Asunto(s)
Luz , Melatonina/metabolismo , Sueño REM/fisiología , Adulto , Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Electroencefalografía , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Factores de Tiempo
16.
J Biol Rhythms ; 14(4): 320-9, 1999 Aug.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10447313

RESUMEN

The accuracy with which a circadian pacemaker can entrain to an environmental 24-h zeitgeber signal depends on (a) characteristics of the entraining signal and (b) response characteristics and intrinsic stability of the pacemaker itself. Position of the sun, weather conditions, shades, and behavioral variations (eye closure, burrowing) all modulate the light signal reaching the pacemaker. A simple model of a circadian pacemaker allows researchers to explore the impact of these factors on pacemaker accuracy. Accuracy is operationally defined as the reciprocal value of the day-to-day standard deviation of the clock times at which a reference phase (0) is reached. For the purpose of this exploration, the authors used a model pacemaker characterized solely by its momentary phase and momentary velocity. The average velocity determines the time needed to complete one pacemaker cycle and, therefore, is inversely proportional to pacemaker period. The model pacemaker responds to light by shifting phase and/or changing its velocity. The authors assumed further that phase and velocity show small random fluctuations and that the velocity is subject to aftereffects. Aftereffects were incorporated mathematically in a term allowing period to contract exponentially to a stable steady-state value, with a time constant of 69 d in the absence of light. The simulations demonstrate that a pacemaker reaches highest accuracy when it responds to light by simultaneous phase shifts and changes of its velocity. Phase delays need to coincide with slowing down and advances with speeding up; otherwise, no synchronization to the zeitgeber occurs. At maximal accuracy, the changes in velocity are such that the average period of the pacemaker under entrained conditions equals 24 h. The results suggest that during entrainment, the pacemaker adjusts its period to 24 h, after which daily phase shifts to compensate for differences between the periods of the zeitgeber and the clock are no longer necessary. On average, phase shifts compensate for maladjustments of phase and velocity changes compensate for maladjustments of period.


Asunto(s)
Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Luz , Modelos Biológicos , Periodicidad , Fotoperiodo , Animales , Relojes Biológicos/fisiología , Relojes Biológicos/efectos de la radiación , Ritmo Circadiano/efectos de la radiación , Simulación por Computador , Condicionamiento Operante , Humanos , Reproducibilidad de los Resultados , Luz Solar
17.
Chronobiol Int ; 16(4): 431-40, 1999 Jul.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10442237

RESUMEN

The daily rhythm in body temperature is thought to be the result of the direct effects of activity and the effects of an endogenous circadian clock. Forced desynchrony (FD) is a tool used in human circadian rhythm research to disentangle endogenous and activity-related effects on daily rhythms. In the present study, we applied an FD protocol to rats. We subjected 8 rats for 5 days to a 20 h forced activity cycle consisting of 10 h of forced wakefulness and 10 h for rest and sleep. The procedure aimed to introduce a 10 h sleep/10 h wake cycle, which period was different from the endogenous circadian (about 24 h) rhythm. Of the variation in the raw body temperature data, 68-77% could be explained by a summation of estimated endogenous circadian cycle and forced activity cycle components of body temperature. Free-running circadian periods of body temperature during FD were similar to free-running periods measured in constant conditions. The applied forced activity cycle reduced clock-related circadian modulation of activity. This reduction of circadian modulation of activity did not affect body temperature. Also, the effects of the forced activity on body temperature were remarkably small.


Asunto(s)
Temperatura Corporal/fisiología , Ritmo Circadiano/fisiología , Actividad Motora/fisiología , Animales , Humanos , Masculino , Fotoperiodo , Ratas , Ratas Wistar , Núcleo Supraquiasmático/fisiología
18.
Biol Psychiatry ; 45(8): 1013-22, 1999 Apr 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10386184

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The aim of the present study was to assess the prevalence of seasonal affective disorder (SAD) in The Netherlands. METHODS: The subjects (n = 5356), randomly selected from community registers, were given the Seasonal Pattern Assessment Questionnaire and the Centre for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale over a period of 13 months. The response rate was 52.6%. RESULTS: Three percent of the respondents met the criteria for winter SAD, 0.1% for summer SAD. The criteria for subsyndromal SAD, a milder form of SAD, were met by 8.5%, 0.3% of whom showed a summer pattern. Younger women received a diagnosis of SAD more often than men or older women. CONCLUSIONS: SAD subjects were significantly more often unemployed or on sick leave than other subjects. Respondents who met winter SAD criteria were significantly more depressed than healthy subjects, in both winter and summer. Finally, month of completion had no influence on the number of subjects meeting the SAD criteria.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/epidemiología , Absentismo , Adolescente , Adulto , Distribución por Edad , Anciano , Ambiente , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Países Bajos/epidemiología , Vigilancia de la Población , Prevalencia , Estudios Prospectivos , Estudios Retrospectivos , Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/diagnóstico , Estaciones del Año , Índice de Severidad de la Enfermedad , Distribución por Sexo , Factores de Tiempo , Trabajo
19.
Biol Psychiatry ; 46(2): 239-46, 1999 Jul 15.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10418699

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: Thirty-eight patients with SAD participated in a light visor study addressing two questions. 1. Can the development of a depressive episode be prevented by daily exposure to bright light started before symptom onset in early fall and continued throughout the winter? 2. Does the light have to be visible in order to have beneficial effects? METHODS: Three groups participated in the study: I (n = 14) received bright white light (2500 lux); II, (n = 15) received infrared light (0.18 lux); III (n = 9, control group) did not receive any light treatment at all. RESULTS: Infrared light is just as effective as bright white light. Both are more effective than the control condition. CONCLUSIONS: Light visors can be effectively used to prevent the development of SAD. The fact that exposure to infrared light was as effective as exposure to bright white light questions the specific role of visible light in the treatment of SAD.


Asunto(s)
Rayos Infrarrojos , Fototerapia/instrumentación , Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/prevención & control , Adulto , Diseño de Equipo , Femenino , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , Servicios Preventivos de Salud/métodos
20.
J Affect Disord ; 53(1): 35-48, 1999 Apr.
Artículo en Inglés | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-10363665

RESUMEN

BACKGROUND: The aim of the study is to investigate the relationship between the prevalence of SAD and latitude. METHODS: An overview of the epidemiological literature on the prevalence of SAD is given and studies relevant for the latitudinal dependency of prevalence will be analyzed and discussed. RESULTS: The mean prevalence of SAD is two times higher in North America compared to Europe. Over all prevalence studies, the correlation between prevalence and latitude was not significant. A significant positive correlation was found between prevalence and latitude in North America. For Europe there was a trend in the same direction. CONCLUSIONS: The influence of latitude on prevalence seems to be small and other factors like climate, genetic vulnerability and social-cultural context can be expected to play a more important role. Additional controlled studies taking these factors into account are necessary to identify their influence.


Asunto(s)
Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/psicología , Adulto , Anciano , Niño , Europa (Continente)/epidemiología , Femenino , Geografía , Humanos , Masculino , Persona de Mediana Edad , América del Norte/epidemiología , Prevalencia , Trastorno Afectivo Estacional/epidemiología
SELECCIÓN DE REFERENCIAS
DETALLE DE LA BÚSQUEDA